Are Extended Twin Family Designs Worth the Trouble? A Comparison of the Bias, Precision, and Accuracy of Parameters Estimated in Four Twin Family Models

The classical twin design (CTD) uses observed covariances from monozygotic and dizygotic twin pairs to infer the relative magnitudes of genetic and environmental causes of phenotypic variation. Despite its wide use, it is well known that the CTD can produce biased estimates if its stringent assumptions are not met. By modeling observed covariances of twins’ relatives in addition to twins themselves, extended twin family designs (ETFDs) require less stringent assumptions, can estimate many more parameters of interest, and should produce less biased estimates than the CTD. However, ETFDs are more complicated to use and interpret, and by attempting to estimate a large number of parameters, the precision of parameter estimates may suffer. This paper is a formal investigation into a simple question: Is it worthwhile to use more complex models such as ETFDs in behavioral genetics? In particular, we compare the bias, precision, and accuracy of estimates from the CTD and three increasingly complex ETFDs. We find the CTD does a decent job of estimating broad sense heritability, but CTD estimates of shared environmental effects and the relative importance of additive versus non-additive genetic variance can be biased, sometimes wildly so. Increasingly complex ETFDs, on the other hand, are more accurate and less sensitive to assumptions than simpler models. We conclude that researchers interested in characterizing the environment or the makeup of genetic variation should use ETFDs when possible.

[1]  R. Fisher XV.—The Correlation between Relatives on the Supposition of Mendelian Inheritance. , 1919, Transactions of the Royal Society of Edinburgh.

[2]  W. Nance,et al.  Genetic models for the analysis of data from the families of identical twins. , 1976, Genetics.

[3]  P. Todd,et al.  Mate choice turns cognitive , 1998, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[4]  Hans J. Eysenck,et al.  Genes, culture, and personality : an empirical approach , 1989 .

[5]  M. Neale,et al.  Modeling Extended Twin Family Data I: Description of the Cascade Model , 2009, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[6]  Fulker Dw Extensions of the classical twin method. , 1982 .

[7]  G. Carey,et al.  Cholesky Problems , 2005, Behavior genetics.

[8]  N. Martin,et al.  A model system for analysis of family resemblance in extended kinships of twins , 1994, Behavior genetics.

[9]  Michael D. Hendy,et al.  Mutation and Evolutionary Rates in Adélie Penguins from the Antarctic , 2008, PLoS genetics.

[10]  W. E. Ritter AS TO THE CAUSES OF EVOLUTION. , 1923, Science.

[11]  D. Grayson Twins reared together: Minimizing shared environmental effects , 1989, Behavior genetics.

[12]  D. Roff,et al.  Dominance variance: associations with selection and fitness , 1995, Heredity.

[13]  L. Eaves Putting the ‘Human’ Back in Genetics: Modeling the Extended Kinships of Twins , 2009, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[14]  D. Fulker,et al.  A bivariate path analysis of fear data on twins and their parents. , 1984, Acta geneticae medicae et gemellologiae.

[15]  P. Wahlberg Chicken Genomics - Linkage and QTL mapping , 2009 .

[16]  G. Casella,et al.  Statistical Inference , 2003, Encyclopedia of Social Network Analysis and Mining.

[17]  Barbara Gregg,et al.  Human Assortative Mating and Genetic Equilibrium: An Evolutionary Perspective , 1980 .

[18]  N. Martin,et al.  A twin-pronged attack on complex traits , 1997, Nature Genetics.

[19]  Judith Berman,et al.  Haplotype Mapping of a Diploid Non-Meiotic Organism Using Existing and Induced Aneuploidies , 2007, PLoS genetics.

[20]  N. Martin,et al.  Model-fitting approaches to the analysis of human behaviour , 1978, Heredity.

[21]  C. Robert Cloninger,et al.  Multifactorial inheritance with cultural transmission and assortative mating. II. a general model of combined polygenic and cultural inheritance. , 1979, American journal of human genetics.

[22]  Sewall Wright,et al.  Fisher's Theory of Dominance , 1929, The American Naturalist.

[23]  J. Tschann,et al.  Brief report: associations of parental warmth, peer support, and gender with adolescent emotional distress. , 2006, Journal of adolescence.

[24]  N. Martin,et al.  Flexible Mx Specification of Various Extended Twin Kinship Designs , 2009, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[25]  M. Keller,et al.  Quantifying and Addressing Parameter Indeterminacy in the Classical Twin Design , 2005, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[26]  W. G. Hill,et al.  Data and Theory Point to Mainly Additive Genetic Variance for Complex Traits , 2008, PLoS genetics.

[27]  M. Keller,et al.  Estimating the Extent of Parameter Bias in the Classical Twin Design: A Comparison of Parameter Estimates From Extended Twin-Family and Classical Twin Designs , 2005, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[28]  Dorret I. Boomsma,et al.  A Note on the Statistical Power in Extended Twin Designs , 2000, Behavior genetics.

[29]  O. Kempthorne The correlation between relatives on the supposition of mendelian inheritance , 1968 .

[30]  L. Penrose,et al.  THE CORRELATION BETWEEN RELATIVES ON THE SUPPOSITION OF MENDELIAN INHERITANCE , 2022 .

[31]  N. Pedersen,et al.  Multivariate Models of Mixed Assortment: Phenotypic Assortment and Social Homogamy for Education and Fluid Ability , 2000, Behavior genetics.

[32]  M. Keller,et al.  Modeling Extended Twin Family Data II: Power Associated With Different Family Structures , 2009, Twin Research and Human Genetics.

[33]  D. Fulker Extensions of the classical twin method. , 1982, Progress in clinical and biological research.

[34]  L. Eaves The use of twins in the analysis of assortative mating , 1979, Heredity.

[35]  A C Heath,et al.  The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance: Informativeness of different relationships , 1985, Behavior genetics.