The ability of young, middle-aged and older drivers to inhibit visual and auditory distraction in a driving simulator task

Abstract Driver distraction is one major cause of road traffic accidents. In order to avoid distraction-related accidents it is important to inhibit irrelevant stimuli and unnecessary responses to distractors and to focus on the driving task, especially when unpredictable critical events occur. Since inhibition is a cognitive function that develops until young adulthood and decreases with increasing age, young and older drivers should be more susceptible to distraction than middle-aged drivers. Using a driving simulation, the present study investigated effects of acoustic and visual distracting stimuli on responses to critical events (flashing up brake lights of a car ahead) in young, middle-aged, and older drivers. The task difficulty was varied in three conditions, in which distractors could either be ignored (perception-only), or required a simple response (detection) or a complex Go-/NoGo-response (discrimination). Response times and error rates to the critical event increased when a simultaneous reaction to the distractor was required. This distraction effect was most pronounced in the discrimination condition, in which the participants had to respond to some of the distracting stimuli and to inhibit responses to some other stimuli. Visual distractors had a stronger impact than acoustic ones. While middle-aged drivers managed distractor inhibition even in difficult tasks quite well (i.e., when responses to distracting stimuli had to be suppressed), response times of young and old drivers increased significantly, especially when distractor stimuli had to be ignored. The results demonstrate the high impact of distraction on driving performance in critical traffic situations and indicate a driving-related inhibition deficit in young and old drivers.

[1]  Stephen Kaplan,et al.  The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework , 1995 .

[2]  F. Craik,et al.  Cognition through the lifespan: mechanisms of change , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  W. C. Maples,et al.  The effects of cell phone use on peripheral vision. , 2008, Optometry.

[4]  Michael Falkenstein,et al.  Age-related differences in performance and stimulus processing in dual task situation , 2011, Brain Research.

[5]  Thomas A Dingus,et al.  Estimates of prevalence and risk associated with inattention and distraction based upon in situ naturalistic data. , 2014, Annals of advances in automotive medicine. Association for the Advancement of Automotive Medicine. Annual Scientific Conference.

[6]  Michael A. Regan,et al.  The Effects of Text Messaging on Young Drivers , 2009, Hum. Factors.

[7]  Kristen E Beede,et al.  Engrossed in conversation: the impact of cell phones on simulated driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[8]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Functional Changes and Driving Performance in Older Drivers: Assessment and Interventions , 2016, Geriatrics.

[9]  L. S. Miller,et al.  Age-related decrements in performance on a brief continuous performance test. , 2005, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[10]  Charlene Hallett,et al.  Driver distraction and driver inattention: definition, relationship and taxonomy. , 2011, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[11]  Jeff K. Caird,et al.  Does Talking on a Cell Phone, With a Passenger, or Dialing Affect Driving Performance? An Updated Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Experimental Studies , 2018, Hum. Factors.

[12]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Examining the Impact of Cell Phone Conversations on Driving Using Meta-Analytic Techniques , 2006, Hum. Factors.

[13]  Fred Tam,et al.  Brain activity during driving with distraction: an immersive fMRI study , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[14]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Driven to Distraction: Dual-Task Studies of Simulated Driving and Conversing on a Cellular Telephone , 2001, Psychological science.

[15]  Lynn Hasher,et al.  Working Memory, Comprehension, and Aging: A Review and a New View , 1988 .

[16]  W. Brouwer,et al.  Age differences in divided attention in a simulated driving task. , 1988, Journal of gerontology.

[17]  Crystal A. Franklin,et al.  Impact of distracted driving on safety and traffic flow. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[18]  Yoko Ishigami,et al.  Is a hands-free phone safer than a handheld phone? , 2009, Journal of safety research.

[19]  Pushpa Choudhary,et al.  Mobile phone use during driving: Effects on speed and effectiveness of driver compensatory behaviour. , 2017, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[20]  Lawrence T Lam,et al.  Distractions and the risk of car crash injury: the effect of drivers' age. , 2002, Journal of safety research.

[21]  Matthew Rizzo,et al.  Distracted driving in elderly and middle-aged drivers. , 2012, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[22]  O. Dickhäuser,et al.  Need for cognition, task difficulty, and the formation of performance expectancies. , 2009, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[23]  Jeff K. Caird,et al.  Twelve Practical and Useful Questions about Driving Simulation , 2011 .

[24]  Katja Kircher,et al.  Effects of Electronic Billboards on Driver Distraction , 2013, Traffic injury prevention.

[25]  Paul J. Perry,et al.  Driving performance comparing older versus younger drivers , 2017, Traffic injury prevention.

[26]  A. Gazzaley,et al.  Distinct mechanisms for the impact of distraction and interruption on working memory in aging , 2012, Neurobiology of Aging.

[27]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Conversation Disrupts Change Detection in Complex Traffic Scenes , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[28]  Peter J Cooper,et al.  Turning gap acceptance decision-making: the impact of driver distraction. , 2002, Journal of safety research.

[29]  Carol Holland,et al.  Influence of personal mobile phone ringing and usual intention to answer on driver error. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[30]  C. Scialfa,et al.  Aging and Vigilance: Who Has the Inhibition Deficit? , 2010, Experimental aging research.

[31]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Multiple resources and performance prediction , 2002 .

[32]  Sophie Forster,et al.  Distraction and Mind-Wandering Under Load , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[33]  Christopher D. Wickens,et al.  Multiple Resources and Mental Workload , 2008, Hum. Factors.

[34]  Mark S. Young,et al.  Malleable Attentional Resources Theory: A New Explanation for the Effects of Mental Underload on Performance , 2002, Hum. Factors.

[35]  Thomas J Triggs,et al.  Driver distraction: the effects of concurrent in-vehicle tasks, road environment complexity and age on driving performance. , 2006, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[36]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Age related strategic differences in processing irrelevant information , 2011, Neuroscience Letters.

[37]  Gary Burnett,et al.  Defining Driver Distraction , 2005 .

[38]  A J McKnight,et al.  The effect of cellular phone use upon driver attention. , 1993, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[39]  Stephan Getzmann,et al.  Evaluating Pro- and Re-Active Driving Behavior by Means of the EEG , 2018, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[40]  O. Carsten,et al.  Mitigating the effects of in-vehicle distractions through use of the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm. , 2013, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[41]  C Collet,et al.  Phoning while driving I: a review of epidemiological, psychological, behavioural and physiological studies , 2010, Ergonomics.

[42]  H. Gr Compensatory control in the regulation of human performance under stress and high workload; a cognitive-energetical framework. , 1997 .

[43]  Peter Chapman,et al.  Prefrontal Cortex Activation and Young Driver Behaviour: A fNIRS Study , 2016, PloS one.

[44]  Nazan Aksan,et al.  Naturalistic Distraction and Driving Safety in Older Drivers , 2013, Hum. Factors.

[45]  Stephan Getzmann,et al.  Driver state examination--Treading new paths. , 2016, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[46]  Anne T McCartt,et al.  Cell Phones and Driving: Review of Research , 2006, Traffic injury prevention.

[47]  Frank Drews,et al.  Profiles in Driver Distraction: Effects of Cell Phone Conversations on Younger and Older Drivers , 2004, Hum. Factors.

[48]  M. Falkenstein,et al.  Age-Related Changes of Neural Control Processes and Their Significance for Driving Performance , 2013 .

[49]  F. Craik,et al.  Executive function across the life span. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[50]  E. Wascher,et al.  Effects of Visual and Acoustic Distraction on Driving Behavior and EEG in Young and Older Car Drivers: A Driving Simulation Study , 2018, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[51]  Mark Billinghurst,et al.  A user study of auditory versus visual interfaces for use while driving , 2008, Int. J. Hum. Comput. Stud..

[52]  I. Robertson,et al.  The absent mind: further investigations of sustained attention to response , 1999, Neuropsychologia.

[53]  M. Boucart,et al.  Age-Related Slowing: Perceptuomotor, Decision, or Attention Decline? , 2010, Experimental aging research.

[54]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Attention and Effort , 1973 .

[55]  S. Hsieh,et al.  Stopping ability in younger and older adults: Behavioral and event-related potential , 2017, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[56]  Feng Guo,et al.  Driver crash risk factors and prevalence evaluation using naturalistic driving data , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[57]  Albert Kircher,et al.  Using mobile telephones: cognitive workload and attention resource allocation. , 2004, Accident; analysis and prevention.

[58]  David L. Strayer,et al.  Talking to your car can drive you to distraction , 2016, Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications.