Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action

Although executive control and automatic behavior have often been considered separate and distinct processes, there is strong emerging and convergent evidence that they may in fact be intricately interlinked. In this review, we draw together evidence showing that visual stimuli cause automatic and unconscious motor activation, and how this in turn has implications for executive control. We discuss object affordances, alien limb syndrome, the visual grasp reflex, subliminal priming, and subliminal triggering of attentional orienting. Consideration of these findings suggests automatic motor activation might form an intrinsic part of all behavior, rather than being categorically different from voluntary actions.

[1]  Raymond M Klein,et al.  Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of vision.

[2]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .

[3]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Tight coupling between positive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[5]  Philip M. Merikle,et al.  Measuring the Relative Magnitude of Unconscious Influences , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.

[6]  Michael A. DiSano,et al.  Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  C. Kennard,et al.  Human Medial Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconscious Inhibition of Voluntary Action , 2007, Neuron.

[8]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  H. Bowman,et al.  Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  S. Link,et al.  A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .

[11]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  James L. McClelland,et al.  The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.

[13]  Robert M. McPeek,et al.  Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.

[14]  E. Awh,et al.  Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[15]  D P Munoz,et al.  Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Spatial attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[17]  E. Keller,et al.  Short-term priming, concurrent processing, and saccade curvature during a target selection task in the monkey , 2001, Vision Research.

[18]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[19]  Antimo Buonocore,et al.  Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  James T. Enns,et al.  Updating a Cautionary Tale of Masked Priming: Reply to Klapp (2005). , 2005 .

[21]  D Laming,et al.  Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.

[22]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[23]  Ulrich Mayr,et al.  Conflict, consciousness, and control , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Exogenous spatial cueing modulates subliminal masked priming. , 2007, Acta psychologica.

[25]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[26]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[27]  S. Tipper,et al.  Hand deviations away from visual cues: Indirect evidence for inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.

[28]  Glyn W. Humphreys,et al.  One more cup of coffee for the road: object–action assemblies, response blocking and response capture after frontal lobe damage , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  P. Martini,et al.  Context, not conflict, drives cognitive control. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  J. Rothwell,et al.  Slow frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation affects reaction times, but not priming effects, in a masked prime task , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[31]  E. Maylor,et al.  No difference between conscious and nonconscious visuomotor control: Evidence from perceptual learning in the masked prime task , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.

[32]  Richard M Shiffrin,et al.  Mechanisms of source confusion and discounting in short-term priming: 1. Effects of prime duration and prime recognition , 2002, Memory & cognition.

[33]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Dissociable Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Conscious and Unconscious Control of Behavior , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  M. Botvinick,et al.  Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.

[36]  Terhi Mustonen,et al.  Mapping the identity of a viewed hand in the motor system: evidence from stimulus-response compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  V. Mark Alien hand syndrome , 2008 .

[38]  K. Forster,et al.  Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): still no identification without attention. , 2004, Psychological review.

[39]  R. Ward,et al.  S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation , 2002 .

[40]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[41]  E. Lauber,et al.  Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[42]  K. Yu,et al.  Attentional modulation of sensorimotor processes in the absence of perceptual awareness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[43]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .

[44]  E. Reingold,et al.  Chapter 17 – Using the Saccadic Inhibition Paradigm to Investigate Saccadic Control in Reading , 2003 .

[45]  V. Lamme,et al.  The Flexible Nature of Unconscious Cognition , 2011, PloS one.

[46]  E. Playford,et al.  Alien limb following posterior cerebral artery stroke: Failure to recognize internally generated movements? , 2007, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[47]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Individual Differences in Subconscious Motor Control Predicted by GABA Concentration in SMA , 2010, Current Biology.

[48]  G. Logan,et al.  When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .

[49]  Philippe Boulinguez,et al.  Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Unconsciously Triggered Conflict Adaptation , 2010, PloS one.

[51]  P. McCormick Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[52]  A. Kingstone,et al.  The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .

[53]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[54]  Brian W. Haas,et al.  Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited to concrete stimulus-response associations. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[55]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. , 2010, Acta psychologica.

[56]  Nitin Tandon,et al.  Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.

[57]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[58]  J. Gibson The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .

[59]  G. Logan,et al.  Automaticity of cognitive control: goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[60]  P. Jaśkowski The effect of nonmasking distractors on the priming of motor responses. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  Mask stimulus triggers inhibition in subliminal visuomotor priming , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.

[62]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization , 2001 .

[63]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The Neurophysiology of Response Competition: Motor Cortex Activation and Inhibition following Subliminal Response Priming , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[64]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[65]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of two components of the reach-to-grasp action during object categorisation in visual memory. , 2006, Acta psychologica.

[66]  Thomas Lachmann,et al.  Negative and positive congruence effects in letters and shapes , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[67]  M. Botvinick Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[68]  K. D'ostilio,et al.  Automatic Stimulus-Induced Medial Premotor Cortex Activation without Perception or Action , 2011, PloS one.

[69]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  No conflict control in the absence of awareness , 2011, Psychological research.

[70]  A. Bompas,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition Reveals the Timing of Automatic and Voluntary Signals in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[71]  A. Aron The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.

[72]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[73]  K. D'ostilio,et al.  Dissociation between unconscious motor response facilitation and conflict in medial frontal areas , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.

[74]  G. Logan On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .

[75]  Peter Praamstra,et al.  The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[76]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition in Voluntary and Reflexive Saccades , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[77]  A. Cronin-Golomb,et al.  The alien hand: cases, categorizations, and anatomical correlates. , 2003, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[78]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .

[79]  Stuart T Klapp,et al.  Two versions of the negative compatibility effect: comment on Lleras and Enns (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[80]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[81]  P. Haggard,et al.  The control of saccade trajectories: Direction of curvature depends on prior knowledge of target location and saccade latency , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.

[82]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[83]  J. Tipples Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[84]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Automatic and controlled processing revisited. , 1984, Psychological review.

[85]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons , 2000, Vision Research.

[86]  Walter Schneider,et al.  Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .

[87]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .

[88]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Repetition blindness has a perceptual locus: evidence from online processing of targets in RSVP streams. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[89]  Borís Burle,et al.  Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.

[90]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.

[91]  J. C. Johnston,et al.  Perceptual repetition blindness effects. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[92]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .

[93]  Hartmut Leuthold,et al.  Mechanisms of Priming by Masked Stimuli: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1998 .

[94]  L. Vainio,et al.  Motor inhibition associated with the affordance of briefly displayed objects , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[95]  K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  The Effect of Parkinson's Disease on the Dynamics of On-line and Proactive Cognitive Control during Action Selection , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[96]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[97]  K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al.  Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[98]  Martin Eimer,et al.  Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[99]  M. Eimer,et al.  Locus of Inhibition in the Masked Priming of Response Alternatives , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[100]  M. Eimer,et al.  Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[101]  Alan Kingstone,et al.  Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects , 2003, Cognition.

[102]  Kitty Z. Xu,et al.  Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[103]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Mask-induced priming and the negative compatibility effect. , 2008, Experimental psychology.

[104]  E. Donchin,et al.  Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[105]  P. Jaśkowski,et al.  On the role of mask structure in subliminal priming. , 2005, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.

[106]  Petroc Sumner,et al.  Conflict in object affordance revealed by grip force , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[107]  Alejandro Lleras,et al.  How much like a target can a mask be? Geometric, spatial, and temporal similarity in priming: a reply to Schlaghecken and Eimer (2006). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[108]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[109]  W. Prinz Common mechanisms in perception and action. , 2002 .

[110]  Wilfried Kunde,et al.  Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[111]  M Eimer,et al.  A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[112]  Christine Deruelle,et al.  Positive and negative compatibility effects , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[113]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[114]  P. Sumner Negative and positive masked-priming – implications for motor inhibition , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[115]  G. Logan,et al.  Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[116]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[117]  Rolf Verleger,et al.  Qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[118]  G. Band,et al.  Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation , 2009, Psychological science.

[119]  Yan Huang,et al.  The precedence of topological change over top-down attention in masked priming. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[120]  Philip L. Smith,et al.  Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.

[121]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .

[122]  Dr. Tiwari,et al.  A case of corticobasal degeneration presenting with alien limb syndrome. , 2008, Age and ageing.

[123]  C. Eriksen,et al.  An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .

[124]  S. Anderson,et al.  Attentional processes link perception and action , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[125]  J. Ridley Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .

[126]  M. Husain,et al.  At the Edge of Consciousness : Automatic Motor Activation and Voluntary Control , 2008 .

[127]  R M Shiffrin,et al.  Perception and preference in short-term word priming. , 2001, Psychological review.

[128]  Robert L. West,et al.  Positive and negative congruency effects in masked priming: A neuro-computational model based on representation, attention, and conflict , 2009, Brain Research.

[129]  R. Proctor,et al.  The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[130]  R. W. Kentridge,et al.  Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[131]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .

[132]  Uwe Mattler,et al.  Inhibition and decay of motor and nonmotor priming , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.

[133]  C. Kennard,et al.  Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[134]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.

[135]  John Duncan,et al.  The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.

[136]  Rob Ellis,et al.  Does selecting one visual object from several require inhibition of the actions associated with nonselected objects? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[137]  Simon van Gaal,et al.  Unconscious Activation of the Prefrontal No-Go Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[138]  J. Trojanowski,et al.  Cognitive and motor assessment in autopsy-proven corticobasal degeneration , 2007, Neurology.

[139]  D. Boomsma,et al.  Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.

[140]  G D Logan,et al.  Constraints on strategy construction in a speeded discrimination task. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[141]  E. Reingold,et al.  Saccadic Inhibition and Gaze Contingent Research Paradigms , 2000 .

[142]  H. Spinnler,et al.  Utilisation Behaviour Consequent to Bilateral SMA Softening , 2002, Cortex.

[143]  Wataru Sato,et al.  Attentional shift by gaze is triggered without awareness , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.

[144]  R. West,et al.  A Biologically-Plausible Cognitive Model (BPCM) of Positive and Negative Congruency Effects in Masked Priming , 2009 .

[145]  D. Bub,et al.  Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[146]  J. Decety,et al.  Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[147]  E. Soetens,et al.  Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[148]  H. Bowman,et al.  Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming , 2006 .

[149]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.

[150]  Robert W. Kentridge,et al.  Attention without awareness in blindsight , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[151]  F. Hermens,et al.  Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade curvature , 2010, Vision Research.

[152]  Lari Vainio,et al.  Interrupted Object-Based Updating of Reach Program Leads to a Negative Compatibility Effect , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.

[153]  Robert M McPeek,et al.  Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[154]  E. Hazeltine,et al.  Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[155]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[156]  Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al.  Pre-SMA Gray-matter Density Predicts Individual Differences in Action Selection in the Face of Conscious and Unconscious Response Conflict , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[157]  E. Sinforiani,et al.  Alien hand syndrome in left posterior stroke , 2011, Neurological Sciences.

[158]  A. Aron,et al.  Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[159]  K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al.  Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconsciously Triggered Inhibitory Control , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[160]  Eyal M. Reingold,et al.  SACCADIC INHIBITION IN COMPLEX VISUAL TASKS , 1999 .

[161]  S. T. Klapp,et al.  The negative compatibility effect: unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[162]  D. Maurer,et al.  The oculomotor distractor effect in normal and hemianopic vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[163]  W. Kunde,et al.  Sequential modulations of correspondence effects across spatial dimensions and tasks , 2006, Memory & cognition.

[164]  J. Enns,et al.  Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[165]  H. Heinze,et al.  Alien Hand Syndrome: Neural Correlates of Movements without Conscious Will , 2010, PLoS ONE.