Automatic motor activation in the executive control of action
暂无分享,去创建一个
Petroc Sumner | Masud Husain | Frédéric Boy | M. Husain | P. Sumner | F. Boy | J. McBride | Jennifer McBride
[1] Raymond M Klein,et al. Orienting of attention without awareness is affected by measurement-induced attentional control settings. , 2003, Journal of vision.
[2] S. Tipper,et al. Selective Reaching to Grasp: Evidence for Distractor Interference Effects , 1997 .
[3] Petroc Sumner,et al. Tight coupling between positive and reversed priming in the masked prime paradigm. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[4] Luigi Cattaneo,et al. Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[5] Philip M. Merikle,et al. Measuring the Relative Magnitude of Unconscious Influences , 1995, Consciousness and Cognition.
[6] Michael A. DiSano,et al. Intracranial EEG Reveals a Time- and Frequency-Specific Role for the Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus and Primary Motor Cortex in Stopping Initiated Responses , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[7] C. Kennard,et al. Human Medial Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconscious Inhibition of Voluntary Action , 2007, Neuron.
[8] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Dissociating consciousness from inhibitory control: evidence for unconsciously triggered response inhibition in the stop-signal task. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[9] H. Bowman,et al. Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[10] S. Link,et al. A sequential theory of psychological discrimination , 1975 .
[11] Martin Eimer,et al. Links between conscious awareness and response inhibition: Evidence from masked priming , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[12] James L. McClelland,et al. The time course of perceptual choice: the leaky, competing accumulator model. , 2001, Psychological review.
[13] Robert M. McPeek,et al. Concurrent processing of saccades in visual search , 2000, Vision Research.
[14] E. Awh,et al. Conflict adaptation effects in the absence of executive control , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.
[15] D P Munoz,et al. Neuronal Correlates for Preparatory Set Associated with Pro-Saccades and Anti-Saccades in the Primate Frontal Eye Field , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[16] G. Rizzolatti,et al. Spatial attention and eye movements , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[17] E. Keller,et al. Short-term priming, concurrent processing, and saccade curvature during a target selection task in the monkey , 2001, Vision Research.
[18] R. Ellis,et al. On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[19] Antimo Buonocore,et al. Saccadic inhibition underlies the remote distractor effect , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[20] James T. Enns,et al. Updating a Cautionary Tale of Masked Priming: Reply to Klapp (2005). , 2005 .
[21] D Laming,et al. Autocorrelation of choice-reaction times. , 1979, Acta psychologica.
[22] D. Munoz,et al. Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[23] Ulrich Mayr,et al. Conflict, consciousness, and control , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[24] Jan Theeuwes,et al. Exogenous spatial cueing modulates subliminal masked priming. , 2007, Acta psychologica.
[25] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[26] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[27] S. Tipper,et al. Hand deviations away from visual cues: Indirect evidence for inhibition , 2006, Experimental Brain Research.
[28] Glyn W. Humphreys,et al. One more cup of coffee for the road: object–action assemblies, response blocking and response capture after frontal lobe damage , 2000, Experimental Brain Research.
[29] P. Martini,et al. Context, not conflict, drives cognitive control. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[30] J. Rothwell,et al. Slow frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation affects reaction times, but not priming effects, in a masked prime task , 2003, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[31] E. Maylor,et al. No difference between conscious and nonconscious visuomotor control: Evidence from perceptual learning in the masked prime task , 2008, Consciousness and Cognition.
[32] Richard M Shiffrin,et al. Mechanisms of source confusion and discounting in short-term priming: 1. Effects of prime duration and prime recognition , 2002, Memory & cognition.
[33] Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al. Dissociable Brain Mechanisms Underlying the Conscious and Unconscious Control of Behavior , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[34] R. W. Kentridge,et al. Independent contributions of the orienting of attention, fixation offset and bilateral stimulation on human saccadic latencies , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.
[35] M. Botvinick,et al. Conflict monitoring and cognitive control. , 2001, Psychological review.
[36] Terhi Mustonen,et al. Mapping the identity of a viewed hand in the motor system: evidence from stimulus-response compatibility. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] V. Mark. Alien hand syndrome , 2008 .
[38] K. Forster,et al. Forty-five years after Broadbent (1958): still no identification without attention. , 2004, Psychological review.
[39] R. Ward,et al. S-R correspondence effects of irrelevant visual affordance: Time course and specificity of response activation , 2002 .
[40] R. W. Kentridge,et al. Attended but unseen: Visual attention is not sufficient for visual awareness , 2008, Neuropsychologia.
[41] E. Lauber,et al. Conditional and unconditional automaticity: a dual-process model of effects of spatial stimulus-response correspondence. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[42] K. Yu,et al. Attentional modulation of sensorimotor processes in the absence of perceptual awareness , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[43] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Activation and suppression in conflict tasks: Empirical clarification through distributional analyses. , 2002 .
[44] E. Reingold,et al. Chapter 17 – Using the Saccadic Inhibition Paradigm to Investigate Saccadic Control in Reading , 2003 .
[45] V. Lamme,et al. The Flexible Nature of Unconscious Cognition , 2011, PloS one.
[46] E. Playford,et al. Alien limb following posterior cerebral artery stroke: Failure to recognize internally generated movements? , 2007, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.
[47] Petroc Sumner,et al. Individual Differences in Subconscious Motor Control Predicted by GABA Concentration in SMA , 2010, Current Biology.
[48] G. Logan,et al. When it helps to be misled: Facilitative effects of increasing the frequency of conflicting stimuli in a Stroop-like task , 1979 .
[49] Philippe Boulinguez,et al. Warning signals induce automatic EMG activations and proactive volitional inhibition: evidence from analysis of error distribution in simple RT. , 2008, Journal of neurophysiology.
[50] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Unconsciously Triggered Conflict Adaptation , 2010, PloS one.
[51] P. McCormick. Orienting attention without awareness. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] A. Kingstone,et al. The eyes have it! Reflexive orienting is triggered by nonpredictive gaze , 1998 .
[53] J. Theeuwes,et al. Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[54] Brian W. Haas,et al. Nonconscious influence of masked stimuli on response selection is limited to concrete stimulus-response associations. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] J. Theeuwes,et al. Unconscious attentional orienting to exogenous cues: A review of the literature. , 2010, Acta psychologica.
[56] Nitin Tandon,et al. Roles for the pre-supplementary motor area and the right inferior frontal gyrus in stopping action: Electrophysiological responses and functional and structural connectivity , 2012, NeuroImage.
[57] Petroc Sumner,et al. Unconscious inhibition separates two forms of cognitive control , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[58] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[59] G. Logan,et al. Automaticity of cognitive control: goal priming in response-inhibition paradigms. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[60] P. Jaśkowski. The effect of nonmasking distractors on the priming of motor responses. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[61] Mask stimulus triggers inhibition in subliminal visuomotor priming , 2008, Experimental Brain Research.
[62] R. Ellis,et al. The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization , 2001 .
[63] Peter Praamstra,et al. The Neurophysiology of Response Competition: Motor Cortex Activation and Inhibition following Subliminal Response Priming , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[64] G. Humphreys,et al. Visual affordances direct action: neuropsychological evidence from manual interference. , 1998, Cognitive neuropsychology.
[65] R. Ellis,et al. The potentiation of two components of the reach-to-grasp action during object categorisation in visual memory. , 2006, Acta psychologica.
[66] Thomas Lachmann,et al. Negative and positive congruence effects in letters and shapes , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.
[67] M. Botvinick. Conflict monitoring and decision making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate function , 2007, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.
[68] K. D'ostilio,et al. Automatic Stimulus-Induced Medial Premotor Cortex Activation without Perception or Action , 2011, PloS one.
[69] Wilfried Kunde,et al. No conflict control in the absence of awareness , 2011, Psychological research.
[70] A. Bompas,et al. Saccadic Inhibition Reveals the Timing of Automatic and Voluntary Signals in the Human Brain , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[71] A. Aron. The Neural Basis of Inhibition in Cognitive Control , 2007, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[72] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. The effect of speed-accuracy strategy on response interference control in Parkinson's disease , 2009, Neuropsychologia.
[73] K. D'ostilio,et al. Dissociation between unconscious motor response facilitation and conflict in medial frontal areas , 2012, The European journal of neuroscience.
[74] G. Logan. On the ability to inhibit thought and action , 1984 .
[75] Peter Praamstra,et al. The basal ganglia and inhibitory mechanisms in response selection: evidence from subliminal priming of motor responses in Parkinson's disease. , 2004, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[76] Eyal M. Reingold,et al. Saccadic Inhibition in Voluntary and Reflexive Saccades , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[77] A. Cronin-Golomb,et al. The alien hand: cases, categorizations, and anatomical correlates. , 2003, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[78] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. , 1977 .
[79] Stuart T Klapp,et al. Two versions of the negative compatibility effect: comment on Lleras and Enns (2004). , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[80] F. Lhermitte. 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[81] P. Haggard,et al. The control of saccade trajectories: Direction of curvature depends on prior knowledge of target location and saccade latency , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[82] T. Robbins,et al. Inhibition of subliminally primed responses is mediated by the caudate and thalamus: evidence from functional MRI and Huntington's disease. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[83] J. Tipples. Eye gaze is not unique: Automatic orienting in response to uninformative arrows , 2002, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[84] R. Shiffrin,et al. Automatic and controlled processing revisited. , 1984, Psychological review.
[85] D. E. Irwin,et al. Attentional and oculomotor capture by onset, luminance and color singletons , 2000, Vision Research.
[86] Walter Schneider,et al. Controlled and Automatic Human Information Processing: 1. Detection, Search, and Attention. , 1977 .
[87] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Version unknown SOURCE ( OR PART OF THE FOLLOWING SOURCE ) : Type article Title To head or to heed ? Beyond the surface of selective action inhibition : a review , 2022 .
[88] J. C. Johnston,et al. Repetition blindness has a perceptual locus: evidence from online processing of targets in RSVP streams. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[89] Borís Burle,et al. Executive control in the Simon effect: an electromyographic and distributional analysis , 2002, Psychological research.
[90] Martin Eimer,et al. Motor activation with and without inhibition: Evidence for a threshold mechanism in motor control , 2002, Perception & psychophysics.
[91] J. C. Johnston,et al. Perceptual repetition blindness effects. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[92] S. Baron-Cohen,et al. Gaze Perception Triggers Reflexive Visuospatial Orienting , 1999 .
[93] Hartmut Leuthold,et al. Mechanisms of Priming by Masked Stimuli: Inferences From Event-Related Brain Potentials , 1998 .
[94] L. Vainio,et al. Motor inhibition associated with the affordance of briefly displayed objects , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[95] K. Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. The Effect of Parkinson's Disease on the Dynamics of On-line and Proactive Cognitive Control during Action Selection , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[96] D. E. Irwin,et al. Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .
[97] K Richard Ridderinkhof,et al. Delta plots in the study of individual differences: new tools reveal response inhibition deficits in AD/Hd that are eliminated by methylphenidate treatment. , 2005, Journal of abnormal psychology.
[98] Martin Eimer,et al. Masked prime stimuli can bias “free” choices between response alternatives , 2004, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[99] M. Eimer,et al. Locus of Inhibition in the Masked Priming of Response Alternatives , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.
[100] M. Eimer,et al. Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.
[101] Alan Kingstone,et al. Abrupt onsets and gaze direction cues trigger independent reflexive attentional effects , 2003, Cognition.
[102] Kitty Z. Xu,et al. Inhibition of voluntary saccadic eye movement commands by abrupt visual onsets. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.
[103] Petroc Sumner,et al. Mask-induced priming and the negative compatibility effect. , 2008, Experimental psychology.
[104] E. Donchin,et al. Optimizing the use of information: strategic control of activation of responses. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[105] P. Jaśkowski,et al. On the role of mask structure in subliminal priming. , 2005, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[106] Petroc Sumner,et al. Conflict in object affordance revealed by grip force , 2012, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[107] Alejandro Lleras,et al. How much like a target can a mask be? Geometric, spatial, and temporal similarity in priming: a reply to Schlaghecken and Eimer (2006). , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[108] A. Osman,et al. Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.
[109] W. Prinz. Common mechanisms in perception and action. , 2002 .
[110] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Sequential modulations of stimulus-response correspondence effects depend on awareness of response conflict , 2003, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[111] M Eimer,et al. A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[112] Christine Deruelle,et al. Positive and negative compatibility effects , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[113] R. Ellis,et al. Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.
[114] P. Sumner. Negative and positive masked-priming – implications for motor inhibition , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[115] G. Logan,et al. Proactive adjustments of response strategies in the stop-signal paradigm. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[116] M. Eimer,et al. Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[117] Rolf Verleger,et al. Qualitative differences between conscious and nonconscious processing? On inverse priming induced by masked arrows. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[118] G. Band,et al. Reward Counteracts Conflict Adaptation , 2009, Psychological science.
[119] Yan Huang,et al. The precedence of topological change over top-down attention in masked priming. , 2011, Journal of vision.
[120] Philip L. Smith,et al. Psychology and neurobiology of simple decisions , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[121] C. Eriksen,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance , 2004 .
[122] Dr. Tiwari,et al. A case of corticobasal degeneration presenting with alien limb syndrome. , 2008, Age and ageing.
[123] C. Eriksen,et al. An electromyographic examination of response competition , 1985 .
[124] S. Anderson,et al. Attentional processes link perception and action , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[125] J. Ridley. Studies of Interference in Serial Verbal Reactions , 2001 .
[126] M. Husain,et al. At the Edge of Consciousness : Automatic Motor Activation and Voluntary Control , 2008 .
[127] R M Shiffrin,et al. Perception and preference in short-term word priming. , 2001, Psychological review.
[128] Robert L. West,et al. Positive and negative congruency effects in masked priming: A neuro-computational model based on representation, attention, and conflict , 2009, Brain Research.
[129] R. Proctor,et al. The influence of irrelevant location information on performance: A review of the Simon and spatial Stroop effects , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[130] R. W. Kentridge,et al. Spatial attention speeds discrimination without awareness in blindsight , 2004, Neuropsychologia.
[131] R. Shiffrin,et al. Controlled and automatic human information processing: I , 1977 .
[132] Uwe Mattler,et al. Inhibition and decay of motor and nonmotor priming , 2005, Perception & psychophysics.
[133] C. Kennard,et al. Functional role of the supplementary and pre-supplementary motor areas , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[134] S. Dehaene,et al. Unconscious Masked Priming Depends on Temporal Attention , 2002, Psychological science.
[135] John Duncan,et al. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control , 2010, NeuroImage.
[136] Rob Ellis,et al. Does selecting one visual object from several require inhibition of the actions associated with nonselected objects? , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[137] Simon van Gaal,et al. Unconscious Activation of the Prefrontal No-Go Network , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[138] J. Trojanowski,et al. Cognitive and motor assessment in autopsy-proven corticobasal degeneration , 2007, Neurology.
[139] D. Boomsma,et al. Conditional accuracy in response interference tasks: Evidence from the Eriksen flanker task and the spatial conflict task , 2008, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[140] G D Logan,et al. Constraints on strategy construction in a speeded discrimination task. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[141] E. Reingold,et al. Saccadic Inhibition and Gaze Contingent Research Paradigms , 2000 .
[142] H. Spinnler,et al. Utilisation Behaviour Consequent to Bilateral SMA Softening , 2002, Cortex.
[143] Wataru Sato,et al. Attentional shift by gaze is triggered without awareness , 2007, Experimental Brain Research.
[144] R. West,et al. A Biologically-Plausible Cognitive Model (BPCM) of Positive and Negative Congruency Effects in Masked Priming , 2009 .
[145] D. Bub,et al. Grasping beer mugs: on the dynamics of alignment effects induced by handled objects. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[146] J. Decety,et al. Does visual perception of object afford action? Evidence from a neuroimaging study , 2002, Neuropsychologia.
[147] E. Soetens,et al. Control over location-based response activation in the Simon task: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[148] H. Bowman,et al. Dissociating local and global levels of perceptuo-motor control in masked priming , 2006 .
[149] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Anterior Cingulate Conflict Monitoring and Adjustments in Control , 2004, Science.
[150] Robert W. Kentridge,et al. Attention without awareness in blindsight , 1999, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[151] F. Hermens,et al. Inhibition of masked primes as revealed by saccade curvature , 2010, Vision Research.
[152] Lari Vainio,et al. Interrupted Object-Based Updating of Reach Program Leads to a Negative Compatibility Effect , 2009, Journal of motor behavior.
[153] Robert M McPeek,et al. Competition between saccade goals in the superior colliculus produces saccade curvature. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.
[154] E. Hazeltine,et al. Conflict monitoring and feature overlap: Two sources of sequential modulations , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[155] C. Eriksen,et al. Pre- and poststimulus activation of response channels: a psychophysiological analysis. , 1988, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[156] Johannes J. Fahrenfort,et al. Pre-SMA Gray-matter Density Predicts Individual Differences in Action Selection in the Face of Conscious and Unconscious Response Conflict , 2011, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[157] E. Sinforiani,et al. Alien hand syndrome in left posterior stroke , 2011, Neurological Sciences.
[158] A. Aron,et al. Theta burst stimulation dissociates attention and action updating in human inferior frontal cortex , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[159] K. R. Ridderinkhof,et al. Frontal Cortex Mediates Unconsciously Triggered Inhibitory Control , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[160] Eyal M. Reingold,et al. SACCADIC INHIBITION IN COMPLEX VISUAL TASKS , 1999 .
[161] S. T. Klapp,et al. The negative compatibility effect: unconscious inhibition influences reaction time and response selection. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[162] D. Maurer,et al. The oculomotor distractor effect in normal and hemianopic vision , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[163] W. Kunde,et al. Sequential modulations of correspondence effects across spatial dimensions and tasks , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[164] J. Enns,et al. Negative compatibility or object updating? A cautionary tale of mask-dependent priming. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[165] H. Heinze,et al. Alien Hand Syndrome: Neural Correlates of Movements without Conscious Will , 2010, PLoS ONE.