Of mice (Mus musculus) and toddlers (Homo sapiens): evidence for species-general spatial reorientation.

There is ongoing debate in spatial cognition about the mechanisms by which organisms are able to reorient, or reestablish a position, in the world after losing their bearing. The traditional view is that there is an encapsulated reorientation module that can only process environmental geometry such as distances or angles (Cheng, 1986). Recently, this view has been challenged on the grounds that the reorientation mechanism is only able to accept geometric information and may instead depend on a more general ability to use relative cues. J. Huttenlocher and S. F. Lourenco (2007) demonstrated that toddlers are successfully able to reorient with continuous cues but show remarkable deficits using categorical cues that are similar in perceptual complexity for reorientation. Here, the authors show the same pattern of results with mice (Mus musculus). These findings provide evidence that there is a homologous reorientation mechanism between mice and humans. Thus, future researchers can examine the genetic basis of this important cognitive ability.

[1]  Alinda Friedman,et al.  Penetrating the geometric module: catalyzing children's use of landmarks. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[2]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Is language necessary for human spatial reorientation? Reconsidering evidence from dual task paradigms , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.

[3]  Lynn Nadel,et al.  Children's Use of Landmarks: Implications for Modularity Theory , 2002, Psychological science.

[4]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Toddlers' use of metric information and landmarks to reorient. , 2001, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[5]  E. Spelke,et al.  Modularity and development: the case of spatial reorientation , 1996, Cognition.

[6]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  Sources of Flexibility in Human Cognition: Dual-Task Studies of Space and Language , 1999, Cognitive Psychology.

[7]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  1 Explaining the Development of Spatial Reorientation : Modularity-Plus-Language Versus the Emergence of Adaptive Combination , 2007 .

[8]  Allen Cheung,et al.  The information content of panoramic images I: The rotational errors and the similarity of views in rectangular experimental arenas. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  M. Schachner,et al.  Adult but not aged C57BL/6 male mice are capable of using geometry for orientation. , 2006, Learning & memory.

[10]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[11]  John Gibbon,et al.  Categorical Scaling of Duration Bisection in Pigeons (Columba livia), Mice (Mus musculus), and Humans (Homo sapiens) , 2008, Psychological science.

[12]  W. F. Prokasy,et al.  Classical conditioning II: Current research and theory. , 1972 .

[13]  H. Wässle,et al.  The Primordial, Blue-Cone Color System of the Mouse Retina , 2005, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[14]  J. Spencer,et al.  The Emerging Spatial Mind , 2007 .

[15]  K. Cheng A purely geometric module in the rat's spatial representation , 1986, Cognition.

[16]  G. H. Jacobs,et al.  Influence of cone pigment coexpression on spectral sensitivity and color vision in the mouse , 2004, Vision Research.

[17]  Elizabeth S. Spelke,et al.  A geometric process for spatial reorientation in young children , 1994, Nature.

[18]  Neil Burgess,et al.  Children reorient using the left/right sense of coloured landmarks at 18–24 months , 2008, Cognition.

[19]  Stella F. Lourenco,et al.  Coding location in enclosed spaces: is geometry the principle? , 2007, Developmental science.

[20]  Richard E Brown,et al.  The influence of visual ability on learning and memory performance in 13 strains of mice. , 2007, Learning & memory.

[21]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Development of Spatial Cognition , 2007 .

[22]  Allen Cheung,et al.  The information content of panoramic images II: view-based navigation in nonrectangular experimental arenas. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[23]  S. Shettleworth,et al.  Learning about Environmental Geometry: an Associative Model , 2022 .

[24]  N. Newcombe,et al.  Is there a geometric module for spatial orientation? squaring theory and evidence , 2005, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[25]  L. Hermer-Vazquez,et al.  Language, space, and the development of cognitive flexibility in humans: the case of two spatial memory tasks , 2001, Cognition.