Internally Simulated Movement Sensations during Motor Imagery Activate Cortical Motor Areas and the Cerebellum

It has been proposed that motor imagery contains an element of sensory experiences (kinesthetic sensations), which is a substitute for the sensory feedback that would normally arise from the overt action. No evidence has been provided about whether kinesthetic sensation is centrally simulated during motor imagery. We psychophysically tested whether motor imagery of palmar flexion or dorsiflexion of the right wrist would influence the sensation of illusory palmar flexion elicited by tendon vibration. We also tested whether motor imagery of wrist movement shared the same neural substrates involving the illusory sensation elicited by the peripheral stimuli. Regional cerebral blood flow was measured with H215O and positron emission tomography in 10 right-handed subjects. The right tendon of the wrist extensor was vibrated at 83 Hz (“illusion”) or at 12.5 Hz with no illusion (“vibration”). Subjects imagined doing wrist movements of alternating palmar and dorsiflexion at the same speed with the experienced illusory movements (“imagery”). A “rest” condition with eyes closed was included. We identified common active fields between the contrasts of imagery versus rest and illusion versus vibration. Motor imagery of palmar flexion psychophysically enhanced the experienced illusory angles of plamar flexion, whereas dorsiflexion imagery reduced it in the absence of overt movement. Motor imagery and the illusory sensation commonly activated the contralateral cingulate motor areas, supplementary motor area, dorsal premotor cortex, and ipsilateral cerebellum. We conclude that kinesthetic sensation associated with imagined movement is internally simulated during motor imagery by recruiting multiple motor areas.

[1]  G. Holmes THE CEREBELLUM OF MAN , 1939 .

[2]  P. Matthews,et al.  The reflex excitation of the soleus muscle of the decerebrate cat caused by vibration applied to its tendon , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[3]  G. Eklund,et al.  Normal variability of tonic vibration reflexes in man. , 1966, Experimental neurology.

[4]  D. McCloskey,et al.  Proprioceptive Illusions Induced by Muscle Vibration: Contribution by Muscle Spindles to Perception? , 1972, Science.

[5]  D. McCloskey,et al.  The contribution of muscle afferents to kinaesthesia shown by vibration induced illusions of movement and by the effects of paralysing joint afferents. , 1972, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[6]  R. Porter,et al.  Relationship between the activity of precentral neurones during active and passive movements in conscious monkeys , 1976, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B. Biological Sciences.

[7]  D. Burke,et al.  The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration of non‐contracting muscles. , 1976, The Journal of physiology.

[8]  B Craske,et al.  Perception of impossible limb positions induced by tendon vibration. , 1977, Science.

[9]  E. Fetz,et al.  Sensory and motor responses of precentral cortex cells during comparable passive and active joint movements. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  P. Roland,et al.  Supplementary motor area and other cortical areas in organization of voluntary movements in man. , 1980, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  J. B. Preston,et al.  Two representations of the hand in area 4 of a primate. II. Somatosensory input organization. , 1982, Journal of neurophysiology.

[12]  E. Bauswein,et al.  Simple and complex spike activity of cerebellar Purkinje cells during active and passive movements in the awake monkey. , 1983, The Journal of physiology.

[13]  D. Landers,et al.  The effects of mental practice on motor skill learning and performance: A meta-analysis. , 1983 .

[14]  M. Mintun,et al.  A Noninvasive Approach to Quantitative Functional Brain Mapping with H215O and Positron Emission Tomography , 1984, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[15]  F. J. Clark,et al.  Role of intramuscular receptors in the awareness of limb position. , 1985, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  S. Gandevia Illusory movements produced by electrical stimulation of low-threshold muscle afferents from the hand. , 1985, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[17]  Masanobu Ito,et al.  A new test for controllability of motor imagery: the examination of its validity and reliability , 1986 .

[18]  M. Mintun,et al.  Noninvasive functional brain mapping by change-distribution analysis of averaged PET images of H215O tissue activity. , 1989, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[19]  Paul Kinahan,et al.  Analytic 3D image reconstruction using all detected events , 1989 .

[20]  J. Decety,et al.  The cerebellum participates in mental activity: tomographic measurements of regional cerebral blood flow , 1990, Brain Research.

[21]  O B White,et al.  Responses of monkey precentral neurones to passive movements and phasic muscle stretch: relevance to man. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[22]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Functional organization of human supplementary motor cortex studied by electrical stimulation , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[23]  J. Houk,et al.  Movement-related inputs to intermediate cerebellum of the monkey. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[24]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Assessing the significance of focal activations using their spatial extent , 1994, Human brain mapping.

[25]  E Naito,et al.  Controllability of Motor Imagery and Transformation of Visual Imagery , 1994, Perceptual and motor skills.

[26]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[27]  T G Turkington,et al.  Performance characteristics of a whole-body PET scanner. , 1994, Journal of nuclear medicine : official publication, Society of Nuclear Medicine.

[28]  A. Wing,et al.  Coordination of aimed movements in a case of unilateral cerebellar damage , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  M. Hallett,et al.  Disturbances of kinaesthesia in patients with cerebellar disorders. , 1994, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[30]  E Wyllie,et al.  Functional anatomy of the human supplementary sensorimotor area: results of extraoperative electrical stimulation. , 1994, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[32]  Thomas K. Lewellen,et al.  Investigation of the performance of the General Electric Advance positron emission tomograph in 3D mode , 1995 .

[33]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[34]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[35]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Topographical representations of mental images in primary visual cortex , 1995, Nature.

[36]  W. T. Thach,et al.  Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of interaction torques across multiple joints. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[37]  J. Annett On knowing how to do things: a theory of motor imagery. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  K. Zilles,et al.  Functions and structures of the motor cortices in humans , 1996, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[39]  A. Sirigu,et al.  The Mental Representation of Hand Movements After Parietal Cortex Damage , 1996, Science.

[40]  D. F. Collins,et al.  Movement illusions evoked by ensemble cutaneous input from the dorsum of the human hand. , 1996, The Journal of physiology.

[41]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Possible involvement of primary motor cortex in mentally simulated movement: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. , 1996, Neuroreport.

[42]  M. Diamond,et al.  Primary Motor and Sensory Cortex Activation during Motor Performance and Motor Imagery: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[43]  S. Kiebel,et al.  Brain Representation of Active and Passive Movements , 1996, NeuroImage.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Cognitive Conjunction: A New Approach to Brain Activation Experiments , 1997, NeuroImage.

[45]  C Frith,et al.  Brain mechanisms associated with top-down processes in perception. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[46]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Neural Systems Shared by Visual Imagery and Visual Perception: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1997, NeuroImage.

[47]  H. C. Diener,et al.  The Relevance of Sensory Input for the Cerebellar Control of Movements , 1997, NeuroImage.

[48]  T. Kasai,et al.  Evidence for facilitation of motor evoked potentials (MEPs) induced by motor imagery , 1997, Brain Research.

[49]  Peter Herscovitch,et al.  Optimization of Noninvasive Activation Studies with 15O-Water and Three-Dimensional Positron Emission Tomography , 1997, Journal of cerebral blood flow and metabolism : official journal of the International Society of Cerebral Blood Flow and Metabolism.

[50]  M. Hallett,et al.  Cerebral Processes Related to Visuomotor Imagery and Generation of Simple Finger Movements Studied with Positron Emission Tomography , 1998, NeuroImage.

[51]  U Klose,et al.  Comparing motion‐ and imagery‐related activation in the human cerebellum: A functional MRI study , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[52]  K. Zilles,et al.  Illusory Arm Movements Activate Cortical Motor Areas: A Positron Emission Tomography Study , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Erb,et al.  Activation of Cortical and Cerebellar Motor Areas during Executed and Imagined Hand Movements: An fMRI Study , 1999, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[54]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  The role of area 17 in visual imagery: convergent evidence from PET and rTMS. , 1999, Science.

[55]  M. Jeannerod,et al.  Mental imaging of motor activity in humans , 1999, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[56]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  Three-Dimensional MRI Atlas of the Human Cerebellum in Proportional Stereotaxic Space , 1999, NeuroImage.

[57]  K. Amunts,et al.  Broca's region subserves imagery of motion: A combined cytoarchitectonic and fMRI study , 2000, Human brain mapping.

[58]  E. Tulving,et al.  Reactivation of encoding-related brain activity during memory retrieval. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[59]  H. Forssberg,et al.  Simultaneous movements of upper and lower limbs are coordinated by motor representations that are shared by both limbs: a PET study , 2000, The European journal of neuroscience.

[60]  S. Petersen,et al.  Memory's echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[61]  Alan C. Evans,et al.  MRI Atlas of the Human Cerebellum , 2000 .

[62]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[63]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  Motor imagery in normal subjects and in asymmetrical Parkinson’s disease , 2000, Neurology.

[64]  J. Decety,et al.  Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: a PET investigation of agency , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[65]  S P Gandhi Memory retrieval: Reactivating sensory cortex , 2001, Current Biology.

[66]  J. Hunter Downs,et al.  FMRI mapping of the somatosensory cortex with vibratory stimuli Is there a dependency on stimulus frequency? , 2001, Brain Research.

[67]  E Naito,et al.  Kinesthetic illusion of wrist movement activates motor-related areas , 2001, Neuroreport.

[68]  E. Naito,et al.  Perceptual changes in illusory wrist flexion angles resulting from motor imagery of the same wrist movements , 2002, Neuroscience.

[69]  J. Roll,et al.  Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[70]  J. Roll,et al.  Alteration of proprioceptive messages induced by tendon vibration in man: a microneurographic study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.