Tapping the Plate or Hitting the Bottle: Sound and Movement in Self-accompanied and Musician-accompanied Dance

Unravelling correspondences between musical sounds and dance movements can lead to insights into the selection and development of culturally modified sensory, motor and perceptual capacities. Gaining these insights requires the development of holistic methodologies to analyse sound and movement concomitantly. The cultural training of performers and their intersubjective dynamics orient the interactions between sound and movement in any performance genre. As an entry point into considering the intimate entanglement of music and dance, this article opens with an account of the direct correspondences between sound and movement in Afro-Brazilian stick-dancing before analysing the more obscure relationships between sound and movement in West Sumatran plate-dancing. The rhythmic swinging of hand-held plates can be accompanied by tapping sounds created by dancers or by musicians. Differences between the timing of movement-related sounds in self-accompanied and musician-accompanied plate-dancing reveal how humanly organised expression is shaped by the interplay of bodily skill, perceptual processes and cultural experience.

[1]  Catherine J. Stevens,et al.  Minds and motion : dynamical systems in choreography, creativity, and dance , 2005 .

[2]  Flavia Gervasi Les Cahiers de la Société québécoise de recherche en musique , 2022 .

[3]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  Margot D. Lasher The cognitive representation of an event involving human motion , 1981, Cognitive Psychology.

[5]  Ashley Thorpe Only Joking? The Relationship between the Clown and Percussion in Jingju , 2005 .

[6]  Pierre-Emmanuel Lacocque,et al.  On the search for meaning , 1982, Journal of Religion and Health.

[7]  Robert W. Mitchell,et al.  Embodying Music: Matching Music and Dance in Memory , 2001 .

[8]  Morwaread Farbood,et al.  A quantitative, parametric model of musical tension , 2006 .

[9]  M Boltz,et al.  Effects of background music on the remembering of filmed events , 1991, Memory & cognition.

[10]  C. Keysers,et al.  Empathy and the Somatotopic Auditory Mirror System in Humans , 2006, Current Biology.

[11]  S. Jordan Choreomusical Conversations: Facing a Double Challenge , 2011, Dance Research Journal.

[12]  J. Hanna,et al.  To Dance is Human: A Theory of Nonverbal Communication , 1979 .

[13]  H. Minors Music and Movement in Dialogue: Exploring Gesture in Soundpainting , 2012 .

[14]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Building a motor simulation de novo: Observation of dance by dancers , 2006, NeuroImage.

[15]  R. Widdess Musical Structure, Performance and Meaning: The Case of a Stick-Dance from Nepal , 2006 .

[16]  Stephanie Jordan,et al.  Ted Shawn's music visualizations , 1983 .

[17]  Greg Downey,et al.  Listening to Capoeira: Phenomenology, Embodiment, and the Materiality of Music , 2002 .

[18]  M. Leman,et al.  Musical gestures : sound, movement, and meaning , 2010 .

[19]  E. Bigand,et al.  Perceiving musical tension in long chord sequences , 1999, Psychological research.

[20]  Stephanie Jordan,et al.  Agon: A Musical/Choreographic Analysis , 1993, Dance Research Journal.

[21]  John A. Sloboda,et al.  Does Music Mean Anything ? , 1998 .

[22]  Isabella Poggi,et al.  Gestures in performance , 2009 .

[23]  Paul H. Mason,et al.  Music, dance and the total art work: choreomusicology in theory and practice , 2012 .

[24]  D. Burrows A Dynamical Systems Perspective on Music , 1997 .

[25]  Marco Iacoboni,et al.  Multimodal action representation in human left ventral premotor cortex , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[26]  Hans-Jochen Heinze,et al.  Shared networks for auditory and motor processing in professional pianists: Evidence from fMRI conjunction , 2006, NeuroImage.

[27]  C. Krumhansl Music: A Link Between Cognition and Emotion , 2002 .

[28]  H. McGurk,et al.  Hearing lips and seeing voices , 1976, Nature.

[29]  J. Mayer,et al.  Mood inductions for four specific moods: A procedure employing guided imagery vignettes with music , 1995 .

[30]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[31]  Stephanie Jordan,et al.  Moving Music: Dialogues with Music in Twentieth Century Ballet , 2002 .

[32]  J. R. Royce,et al.  The search for meaning. , 1959 .

[33]  Ruth Foster Knowing in my bones , 1976 .

[34]  Joann Wheeler Kealiinohomoku Dance and Self-Accompaniment , 1965 .

[35]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Neural Circuits Underlying Imitation Learning of Hand Actions An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2004, Neuron.

[36]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[37]  J. Torp Musical Movement: Towards a Common Term for Music and Dance , 2013, Yearbook for Traditional Music.

[38]  D. Vézina Phenomenology of dance , 2006 .

[39]  John Kratus A Developmental Study of Children's Interpretation of Emotion in Music , 1993 .

[40]  C. Laughlin Art and Spirit: The Artistic Brain, the Navajo Concept of Hozho, and Kandinsky’s “Inner Necessity” , 2004 .

[41]  H. Aitken The Piece as a Whole: Studies in Holistic Musical Analysis , 1997 .

[42]  Marcelo M. Wanderley,et al.  Music to my eyes: Cross-modal interactions in the perception of emotions in musical performance , 2011, Cognition.

[43]  C. Krumhansl A Perceptual Analysis of Mozart's Piano Sonata K. 282: Segmentation, Tension, and Musical Ideas , 1996 .

[44]  M. Sheets‐Johnstone The phenomenology of dance , 1966 .

[45]  C. Krumhansl,et al.  Can Dance Reflect the Structural and Expressive Qualities of Music? A Perceptual Experiment on Balanchine's Choreography of Mozart's Divertimento No. 15 , 1997 .