Changing stereotype content through mental imagery: Imagining intergroup contact promotes stereotype change
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Stephen M. Garcia,et al. Crowded Minds : The Implicit Bystander Effect , 2002 .
[2] Michael I. Norton,et al. This Old Stereotype: The Pervasiveness and Persistence of the Elderly Stereotype , 2005 .
[3] S. Fiske,et al. Anti-American Sentiment and America's Perceived Intent to Dominate: An 11-Nation Study , 2006 .
[4] F. Barbera,et al. SUBTLE AND BLATANT PREJUDICE TOWARD CHINESE, MOROCCAN, AND POLISH IMMIGRANTS IN AN ITALIAN PROVINCE , 2010 .
[5] Richard J. Crisp,et al. Elaboration enhances the imagined contact effect , 2010 .
[6] Miles Hewstone,et al. Intergroup Contact and Prejudice Toward Immigrants in Italy: The Mediational Role of Anxiety and the Moderational Role of Group Salience , 2003 .
[7] Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al. The BIAS map: behaviors from intergroup affect and stereotypes. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[8] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. Can imagined interactions produce positive perceptions? Reducing prejudice through simulated social contact. , 2009, The American psychologist.
[9] Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al. Warmth and Competence As Universal Dimensions of Social Perception: The Stereotype Content Model and the BIAS Map , 2008 .
[10] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. Behavioural consequences of imagining intergroup contact with stigmatized outgroups , 2012 .
[11] S. Fiske,et al. Not an outgroup, not yet an ingroup: Immigrants in the Stereotype Content Model , 2006 .
[12] S. Fiske,et al. Dehumanizing the Lowest of the Low , 2006, Psychological science.
[13] Rupert Brown,et al. Categorization and intergroup anxiety in contact between British and Japanese nationals , 1999 .
[14] M. Zárate,et al. Cultural threat and perceived realistic group conflict as dual predictors of prejudice , 2004 .
[15] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. From imagery to intention: A dual route model of imagined contact effects , 2010 .
[16] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. Imagining intergroup contact reduces implicit prejudice. , 2010, The British journal of social psychology.
[17] J. Vala,et al. From prejudice to discrimination: The legitimizing role of perceived threat in discrimination against immigrants , 2010 .
[18] John Bell,et al. Changing Attitudes , 1995, ECAI Workshop on Agent Theories, Architectures, and Languages.
[19] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. "Changing attitudes with a little imagination": Imagined contact effects on young children"s intergroup bias , 2011 .
[20] R. Crisp,et al. Attributional processes underlying imagined contact effects , 2011 .
[21] S. Fiske,et al. Social neuroscience evidence for dehumanised perception , 2009 .
[22] M. Hewstone,et al. Fifty-odd years of inter-group contact: from hypothesis to integrated theory. , 2011, The British journal of social psychology.
[23] Rhiannon N. Turner,et al. Imagined Intergroup Contact: Theory, Paradigm and Practice , 2009 .
[24] Linda R. Tropp,et al. A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. , 2006, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[25] Stephen C. Wright,et al. The extended contact effect: Knowledge of cross-group friendships and prejudice. , 1997 .
[26] R. Crisp,et al. Imagined Intergroup Contact: A New Technique for Encouraging Greater Inter-Ethnic Contact in Cyprus , 2010 .
[27] R. Crisp,et al. Imagining intergroup contact promotes projection to outgroups , 2008 .
[28] A. Lenton,et al. Imagining stereotypes away: the moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. , 2001, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[29] Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al. A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. , 2002, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[30] Mark Rubin,et al. Secondary transfer effects from imagined contact: group similarity affects the generalization gradient. , 2011, The British journal of social psychology.
[31] M. Dadds,et al. Imagery in human classical conditioning. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.
[32] Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al. When Being a Model Minority Is Good . . . and Bad: Realistic Threat Explains Negativity Toward Asian Americans , 2008, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[33] Richard J. Crisp,et al. Imagining Intergroup Contact Can Improve Intergroup Attitudes , 2007 .
[34] Miles Hewstone,et al. European Review of Social Psychology , 2012 .
[35] Gordon Hodson. Interracial prison contact: the pros for (socially dominant) cons. , 2008, The British journal of social psychology.
[36] R. Crisp,et al. Mental Simulations of Social Thought and Action , 2011 .
[37] Rupert Brown,et al. Predictors and consequences of negative attitudes toward immigrants in Belgium and Turkey: the role of acculturation preferences and economic competition. , 2007, The British journal of social psychology.
[38] Miles Hewstone,et al. A test of the extended intergroup contact hypothesis: the mediating role of intergroup anxiety, perceived ingroup and outgroup norms, and inclusion of the outgroup in the self. , 2008, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[39] Mark Rubin,et al. Negative Intergroup Contact Makes Group Memberships Salient: Explaining Why Intergroup Conflict Endures , 2010, Personality & social psychology bulletin.
[40] Thomas F. Pettigrew,et al. Future directions for intergroup contact theory and research , 2008 .
[41] Amy J. C. Cuddy,et al. Stereotype content model across cultures: towards universal similarities and some differences. , 2009, The British journal of social psychology.
[42] Gordon Hodson,et al. Independent benefits of contact and friendship on attitudes toward homosexuals among authoritarians and highly identified heterosexuals , 2009 .
[43] J Tudor-Hart,et al. On the nature of prejudice. , 1961, The Eugenics review.
[44] Monica H. Lin,et al. Stereotype Content Model Explains Prejudice for an Envied Outgroup: Scale of Anti-Asian American Stereotypes , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.