Journal of Experimental Psychology : Animal Behavior Processes Conditional Stimulus Informativeness Governs Conditioned Stimulus — Unconditioned Stimulus Associability

[1]  Justin A. Harris,et al.  Response rate and reinforcement rate in Pavlovian conditioning. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[2]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Time and Associative Learning. , 2010, Comparative cognition & behavior reviews.

[3]  A. Dickinson Inaugural editorial for Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes. , 2010 .

[4]  Ryan D Ward,et al.  Timing and anticipation: conceptual and methodological approaches , 2009, The European journal of neuroscience.

[5]  Cornering the Fear Engram: Long-Term Synaptic Changes in the Lateral Nucleus of the Amygdala after Fear Conditioning , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.

[7]  Daniel A. Gottlieb Is the number of trials a primary determinant of conditioned responding? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[8]  P. Holland,et al.  The influence of CS-US interval on several different indices of learning in appetitive conditioning. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[9]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Pavlovian contingencies and temporal information. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[10]  P. Couvillon,et al.  Temporal control of conditioned responding in goldfish. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[11]  C. Gallistel,et al.  The learning curve: implications of a quantitative analysis. , 2004, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[12]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Sources of variability and systematic error in mouse timing behavior. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[13]  Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Tracking of the expected time to reinforcement in temporal conditioning procedures , 2003, Learning & behavior.

[14]  P. Balsam,et al.  Timing at the Start of Associative Learning , 2002 .

[15]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Fear conditioning and LTP in the lateral amygdala are sensitive to the same stimulus contingencies , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[16]  M. Mauk,et al.  Latent Acquisition of Timed Responses in Cerebellar Cortex , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[17]  Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Independent effects of stimulus and cycle duration in conditioning: The role of timing processes , 2000 .

[18]  P. Holland Trial and intertrial durations in appetitive conditioning in rats , 2000 .

[19]  Kimberly S. Kirkpatrick,et al.  Stimulus and temporal cues in classical conditioning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[20]  C. Gallistel,et al.  Time, rate, and conditioning. , 2000, Psychological review.

[21]  K. Lattal,et al.  Trial and intertrial durations in Pavlovian conditioning: issues of learning and performance. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[22]  J. Gibbon,et al.  Temporal control during maintenance and extinction of conditioned keypecking in ring doves , 1999 .

[23]  Ralph R. Miller,et al.  Time as content in Pavlovian conditioning , 1998, Behavioural Processes.

[24]  R. R. Miller,et al.  Temporal encoding as a determinant of overshadowing. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[25]  M. McKERNAN,et al.  Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro , 1997, Nature.

[26]  Joseph E LeDoux,et al.  Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala , 1997, Nature.

[27]  P. Milner,et al.  Preconceptions and prerequisites: Understanding the function of synaptic plasticity will also depend on a better systems-level understanding of the multiple types of memory , 1997, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[28]  R. Bevins,et al.  One-trial context fear conditioning as a function of the interstimulus interval , 1995 .

[29]  R. R. Miller,et al.  Temporal encoding as a determinant of blocking. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[30]  M. Davis,et al.  Temporal specificity of fear conditioning: effects of different conditioned stimulus-unconditioned stimulus intervals on the fear-potentiated startle effect. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[31]  E. Kehoe,et al.  Temporal patterns of the rabbit's nictitating membrane response to compound and component stimuli under mixed CS-US intervals. , 1989, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  P. Killeen,et al.  A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.

[33]  R. F. Thompson,et al.  Modeling the neural substrates of associative learning and memory: a computational approach. , 1987, Psychological review.

[34]  P. Shizgal,et al.  A portrait of the substrate for self-stimulation. , 1981, Psychological review.

[35]  J. Pearce,et al.  A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. , 1980, Psychological review.

[36]  H. Terrace,et al.  Trial and intertrial durations in autoshaping. , 1977 .

[37]  J. Gibbon Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .

[38]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[39]  R. Church,et al.  Magnitude of fear as a function of expected time to all aversive event , 1974 .

[40]  R. Rescorla,et al.  A theory of Pavlovian conditioning : Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement , 1972 .

[41]  S. R. Coleman,et al.  Classical conditioning of the rabbit's (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane response under symmetrical CS-US interval shifts. , 1971, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[42]  M. M. Patterson Classical conditioning of the rabbit's (Oryctolagus cuniculus) nictitating membrane response with fluctuating ISI and intracranial CS. , 1970, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[43]  G. S. Reynolds,et al.  A quantitative analysis of the responding maintained by interval schedules of reinforcement. , 1968, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.

[44]  R. Rescorla Inhibition of delay in Pavlovian fear conditioning. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[45]  R. Rescorla,et al.  Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. , 1967, Psychological review.

[46]  M. Bitterman,et al.  EFFECT OF PARTIAL REINFORCEMENT ON THE EXTINCTION OF A CLASSICALLY CONDITIONED RESPONSE IN THE GOLDFISH. , 1965, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[47]  S. Levine,et al.  Temporal factors in avoidance learning. , 1960, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[48]  L. Kamin Acquisition of avoidance with a variable CS-US INTERVAL. , 1960, Canadian journal of psychology.