1. Colonies of wild Rattus norvegicus Berkenhout were kept in closed rooms containing nesting sites. The conditions permitted breeding. 2. Seventy-six experiments, each lasting 6–35 days, were done to test the rats' food preferences. In each experiment except two, a choice was given between two foods, both available in excess. In the two exceptional experiments a choice between three foods was given. 3. Although marked preferences were shown, a food was rarely quite rejected in favour of an alternative. 4. Among cereals, wholemeal was preferred to wheat grains, but wheat grains were preferred to white flour; little or no white flour was eaten if wholemeal was available. Wheat germ was eaten only in small quantities when an alternative cereal was available, but in larger quantities if the alternative was sugar or a sugar-cereal mixture. 5. The addition of sugar or saccharin to cereals increased acceptability; sugar was preferred to saccharin. 6. The addition of arachis oil to wholemeal or wheat increased acceptability. 7. Cod-liver oil at 2·4 % acted as a deterrent. 8. Total calorific intake remained steady over short periods, but over several weeks rose as the mass of the rat colony increased. The higher the mean weight of the rats in a colony, the lower the calorific intake per unit weight of rat. 9. The amount of food eaten was partly determined by its calorigenic capacity: in particular, wet foods were eaten in much larger quantities than dry foods. 10. The food preferences of wild rats differ in several respects from those of white Rattus norvegicus. 11. Apart from palatability or flavour, the main factors influencing food preference were (a) previous experience and probably (b) the nutritional value of the food. The effect of previous experience was often observed when rats, accustomed to one food, gradually changed from the accustomed food to another. The stages of a change to a new food, or the establishment of a particular preference, varied, and are discussed in detail.
[1]
M. M. Spencer.
The behaviour of rats and mice feeding on whole grains
,
1953,
Journal of Hygiene.
[2]
M. M. Spencer,et al.
Responses of wild rats to offensive smells and tastes
,
1953
.
[3]
S. A. Barnett,et al.
The action of dicoumarol on laboratory and wild rats, and its effect on feeding behaviour
,
1950,
Journal of Hygiene.
[4]
T. Moore.
Imbalance of Fat-Soluble Vitamins
,
1949,
British Journal of Nutrition.
[5]
J. H. Kenneth.
Sense of Taste
,
1949
.
[6]
A. J. Carlson,et al.
Influence of Texture of Food on Its Acceptance by Rats.
,
1949,
Science.
[7]
J. H. Kenneth.
The Taste Sense and the Relative Sweetness of Sugars and other Sweet Substances
,
1949,
Nature.
[8]
E. Scott,et al.
Self selection of diet: the nature of appetites for B vitamins.
,
1947,
The Journal of nutrition.
[9]
E. Adolph.
Urges to eat and drink in rats.
,
1947,
The American journal of physiology.
[10]
E. Scott,et al.
Self selection of diet; appetite for carbohydrates.
,
1947,
The Journal of nutrition.
[11]
E. Scott,et al.
Self selection of diet; appetite for B vitamins.
,
1946,
The Journal of nutrition.
[12]
E. Scott,et al.
Self selection of diet; selection of purified components.
,
1946,
The Journal of nutrition.
[13]
D. Chitty,et al.
Techniques for the study of the Norway rat.
,
1946,
Journal of mammalogy.
[14]
M. Hausmann.
The behavior of albino rats in choosing foods II Differentiation between sugar and saccharin
,
1933
.
[15]
M. Hausmann.
The behavior of albino rats in choosing foods and stimulants.
,
1932
.
[16]
L. Harris,et al.
"Hypervitaminosis" and "vitamin balance".
,
1928,
The Biochemical journal.