Costs of memory: lessons from ‘mini’ brains
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] Irina Sinakevitch,et al. Ground plan of the insect mushroom body: Functional and evolutionary implications , 2009, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[2] R. Huber,et al. Fish Brains: Evolution and Anvironmental Relationships , 1998, Reviews in Fish Biology and Fisheries.
[3] J. Bolhuis,et al. Memory and brain in food-storing birds: Space oddities or adaptive specializations? , 2008 .
[4] Kate E. Jones,et al. Mating system and brain size in bats , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[5] S. Laughlin,et al. Fly Photoreceptors Demonstrate Energy-Information Trade-Offs in Neural Coding , 2007, PLoS biology.
[6] Randall D. Beer,et al. The Dynamics of Active Categorical Perception in an Evolved Model Agent , 2003, Adapt. Behav..
[7] Heikki Hirvonen,et al. Significance of memory properties in prey choice decisions , 1999 .
[8] T. Kawecki. Evolutionary ecology of learning: insights from fruit flies , 2009, Population Ecology.
[9] R. Barton. Visual specialization and brain evolution in primates , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[10] K. Cheng,et al. Honeybees (Apis mellifera) holding on to memories: response competition causes retroactive interference effects , 2006, Animal Cognition.
[11] R Dukas,et al. Costs of memory: ideas and predictions. , 1999, Journal of theoretical biology.
[12] J. Kaas,et al. Cellular scaling rules for primate brains , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[13] Robin I. M. Dunbar,et al. Both social and ecological factors predict ungulate brain size , 2006, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[14] Tadeusz J. Kawecki,et al. An operating cost of learning in Drosophila melanogaster , 2004, Animal Behaviour.
[15] Tim Tully,et al. Drosophila learning and memory revisited , 1987, Trends in Neurosciences.
[16] K. Safi,et al. Comparative studies of brain evolution: a critical insight from the Chiroptera , 2009, Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society.
[17] R. Dukas,et al. Cognitive ecology : the evolutionary ecology of information processing and decision making , 1998 .
[18] C. Brandes. Genetic differences in learning behavior in honeybees (Apis mellifera capensis) , 1991, Behavior genetics.
[19] A. Agrawal,et al. Re-evaluating the costs and limits of adaptive phenotypic plasticity , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[20] B. Smith,et al. Sensitivity to a change in reward is heritable in the honeybee, Apis mellifera , 2001, Animal Behaviour.
[21] M. Srinivasan,et al. The concepts of ‘sameness’ and ‘difference’ in an insect , 2001, Nature.
[22] Peter N. C. Mohr,et al. Genetic variation in dopaminergic neuromodulation influences the ability to rapidly and flexibly adapt decisions , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[23] A I Houston,et al. Memory and the efficient use of information. , 1987, Journal of theoretical biology.
[24] R. Greenspan,et al. Cognitive consonance: complex brain functions in the fruit fly and its relatives , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[25] T. Kawecki,et al. Reduced learning ability as a consequence of evolutionary adaptation to nutritional stress in Drosophila melanogaster , 2008 .
[26] David F. Sherry,et al. Interval Timing by an Invertebrate, the Bumble Bee Bombus impatiens , 2006, Current Biology.
[27] Lars Chittka,et al. Sensorimotor learning in bumblebees: long-term retention and reversal training , 1998 .
[28] M. Bouton. Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of Pavlovian learning. , 1993, Psychological bulletin.
[29] Wulfila Gronenberg,et al. Reduction of Brain Volume Correlates with Behavioral Changes in Queen Ants , 2002, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[30] Troy Zars,et al. Behavioral functions of the insect mushroom bodies , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[31] Jerry Hirsch,et al. Selection for conditionability in Drosophila melanogaster , 1992 .
[32] Frederic Mery,et al. THE EFFECT OF LEARNING ON EXPERIMENTAL EVOLUTION OF RESOURCE PREFERENCE IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER , 2004, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[33] J. Wixted. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.
[34] S. Laughlin. Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory information , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[35] F. Mery,et al. A natural genetic polymorphism affects retroactive interference in Drosophila melanogaster , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[36] P H Harvey,et al. Comparing brains. , 1990, Science.
[37] J. Sivinski. Mushroom body development in nymphalid butterflies: A correlate of learning? , 1989, Journal of Insect Behavior.
[38] S. Ott,et al. Gregarious desert locusts have substantially larger brains with altered proportions compared with the solitarious phase , 2010, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[39] A Switch from Cycloheximide-Resistant Consolidated Memory to Cycloheximide-Sensitive Reconsolidation and Extinction in Drosophila , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[40] P. Lindenfors. Neocortex evolution in primates: the ‘social brain’ is for females , 2005, Biology Letters.
[41] Simon B. Laughlin,et al. Action Potential Energy Efficiency Varies Among Neuron Types in Vertebrates and Invertebrates , 2010, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[42] S. Farris. Evolutionary Convergence of Higher Brain Centers Spanning the Protostome-Deuterostome Boundary , 2008, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[43] X. Cerdá,et al. Behavioral repertoire of the antCataglyphis cursor (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): Is it possible to elaborate a standard specific one? , 1991, Journal of Insect Behavior.
[44] M. Bastian,et al. Life history costs and benefits of encephalization: a comparative test using data from long-term studies of primates in the wild. , 2008, Journal of human evolution.
[45] Frederic Mery,et al. A Cost of Long-Term Memory in Drosophila , 2005, Science.
[46] T. Kawecki,et al. Learning Ability and Longevity: A Symmetrical Evolutionary Trade-Off in Drosophila , 2008, Evolution; international journal of organic evolution.
[47] Jan Wessnitzer,et al. A model of associative learning in the mushroom body , 2008, Biological Cybernetics.
[48] V. Braithwaite,et al. Effects of predation pressure on the cognitive ability of the poeciliid Brachyraphis episcopi , 2005 .
[49] E. Mallon,et al. No evidence for an evolutionary trade-off between learning and immunity in a social insect , 2009, Biology Letters.
[50] Lars Chittka,et al. Can honey bees count landmarks? , 1995, Animal Behaviour.
[51] T. Carew,et al. Intermediate-term processes in memory formation , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[52] L. Aiello,et al. The Expensive-Tissue Hypothesis: The Brain and the Digestive System in Human and Primate Evolution , 1995, Current Anthropology.
[53] C. M. Lessells,et al. The Evolution of Life Histories , 1994 .
[54] S. Farris. Structural, Functional and Developmental Convergence of the Insect Mushroom Bodies with Higher Brain Centers of Vertebrates , 2008, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[55] R. Menzel. Searching for the memory trace in a mini-brain, the honeybee. , 2001, Learning & memory.
[56] S. Shettleworth. Cognition, evolution, and behavior , 1998 .
[57] Lars Chittka,et al. The correlation of learning speed and natural foraging success in bumble-bees , 2008, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[58] Mandyam V. Srinivasan,et al. Grouping of visual objects by honeybees , 2004, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[59] L. Chittka,et al. Flower Constancy, Insect Psychology, and Plant Evolution , 1999, Naturwissenschaften.
[60] Nicola S Clayton,et al. A test of the adaptive specialization hypothesis: population differences in caching, memory, and the hippocampus in black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapilla). , 2002, Behavioral neuroscience.
[61] I. Coolen,et al. Public Versus Personal Information for Mate Copying in an Invertebrate , 2009, Current Biology.
[62] R. Dukas. Learning decreases heterospecific courtship and mating in fruit flies , 2008, Biology Letters.
[63] D. Reznick. Costs of reproduction: an evaluation of the empirical evidence , 1985 .
[64] M. Takahata,et al. Further exploration into the adaptive design of the arthropod “microbrain”: I. Sensory and memory-processing systems , 2004, Zoological science.
[65] Jerry Hirsch,et al. Conditioned discrimination in the blow fly,Phormia regina: Controls and bidirectional selection , 1984 .
[66] Tim Tully,et al. Dissection of memory formation: from behavioral pharmacology to molecular genetics , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.
[67] S. Farris,et al. Coevolution of generalist feeding ecologies and gyrencephalic mushroom bodies in insects. , 2005, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[68] M. Srinivasan,et al. Evidence for counting in insects , 2008, Animal Cognition.
[69] Wulfila Gronenberg,et al. Brain Allometry and Neural Plasticity in the Bumblebee Bombus occidentalis , 2010, Brain, Behavior and Evolution.
[70] C. Goodman. Isogenic grasshoppers: Genetic variability in the morphology of identified neurons , 1978, The Journal of comparative neurology.
[71] Yadin Dudai,et al. Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused , 2006, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[72] Daniel Sol,et al. Brain Size Predicts the Success of Mammal Species Introduced into Novel Environments , 2008, The American Naturalist.
[73] R. Dukas,et al. Learning improves growth rate in grasshoppers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[74] Low mass–specific brain Na+/K+–ATPase activity in elasmobranch compared to teleost fishes: implications for the large brain size of elasmobranchs , 2000, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[75] Frederic Mery,et al. A fitness cost of learning ability in Drosophila melanogaster , 2003, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[76] J. Niven,et al. Are Bigger Brains Better? , 2009, Current Biology.
[77] J. Ratcliffe. Neuroecology and diet selection in phyllostomid bats , 2009, Behavioural Processes.
[78] G. Amdam,et al. Honeybee Associative Learning Performance and Metabolic Stress Resilience Are Positively Associated , 2010, PloS one.
[79] M. Pie. Behavioral Repertoire, Age Polyethism and Adult Transport in Ectatomma opaciventre (Formicidae: Ponerinae) , 2004, Journal of Insect Behavior.
[80] T. Johnston. Selective costs and benefits of in the evolution of learning , 1996 .
[81] Masakazu Takahata,et al. Exploration into the Adaptive Design of the Arthropod “Microbrain” , 1999 .
[82] Simon B. Laughlin,et al. Visual ecology and voltage-gated ion channels in insect photoreceptors , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.
[83] T. Préat,et al. Genetic dissection of consolidated memory in Drosophila , 1994, Cell.
[84] T. C. Ings,et al. Unterschiede im Lernverhalten zwischen Kolonien einer freilebenden Britischen Hummelpopulation (Hymenoptera: Apidae: Bombus terrestris audax) , 2006 .
[85] D. Stephens,et al. Components of change in the evolution of learning and unlearned preference , 2009, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[86] S. Laughlin,et al. Energy limitation as a selective pressure on the evolution of sensory systems , 2008, Journal of Experimental Biology.
[87] R. Williams,et al. The control of neuron number. , 1988, Annual review of neuroscience.
[88] S. Reader,et al. Large‐brained mammals live longer , 2010, Journal of evolutionary biology.
[89] L. Kaiser,et al. Genetic Variability of Conditioned Probing Responses to a Fruit Odor in Leptopilina boulardi(Hymenoptera: Eucoilidae), a DrosophilaParasitoid , 1998, Behavior genetics.
[90] Robert Plomin,et al. The genetics of G in human and mouse , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[91] Candy Rowe,et al. A critique of comparative studies of brain size , 2007, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.