On the consistency of cognitive load

Abstract There are many ways to induce cognitive load. In this paper, we manipulate cognitive capacity using four common techniques: a number memorization task, a visual pattern task, an auditory recall task, and time pressure. Under each load manipulation (as well as under ‘no load’), every participant completes a series of math problems, lottery tasks, logic puzzles, and allocation decisions. We find similar behavioral responses across all techniques: poorer performance on the math problems and logic puzzles, more risk aversion in the lottery tasks, and no systematic impact on allocation decisions. Using within-subject variation, we show that individuals whose math performance is most impacted for a given load manipulation (number memorization), are the same individuals whose performance is most impacted by other load manipulation, and in the other tasks. We also find that participants who scored above the median in a cognitive reflection test (CRT), and are thus able to resist the first response that comes to mind, are greatly impacted when placed under cognitive load; those scoring below the median in the CRT are not impacted much.

[1]  Keith E. Stanovich,et al.  Assessing miserly information processing: An expansion of the Cognitive Reflection Test , 2014 .

[2]  Pablo Brañas-Garza,et al.  Cognitive reflection test: Whom, how, when , 2019, Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics.

[3]  H. Uhlig,et al.  Cognitive Load Increases Risk Aversion , 2016 .

[4]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[5]  G. A. Miller THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .

[6]  Cary A. Deck,et al.  The Effects of Alcohol Use on Economic Decision Making , 2017 .

[7]  Hauke R. Heekeren,et al.  Performance level modulates adult age differences in brain activation during spatial working memory , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[8]  W. Güth,et al.  Being of two minds: Ultimatum offers under cognitive constraints , 2011 .

[9]  A. Rubinstein A Typology of Players: Between Instinctive and Contemplative , 2016 .

[10]  Jörg Oechssler,et al.  Cooling-Off in Negotiations - Does it Work? , 2008 .

[11]  The effect of fast and slow decisions on risk taking , 2017, Journal of risk and uncertainty.

[12]  P. Whitney,et al.  Impulsive decision making and working memory. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[13]  B. Shiv,et al.  Heart and Mind in Conflict: The Interplay of Affect and Cognition in Consumer Decision Making , 1999 .

[14]  P. Whitney,et al.  Framing effects under cognitive load: The role of working memory in risky decisions , 2008, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[15]  D. Heeger Normalization of cell responses in cat striate cortex , 1992, Visual Neuroscience.

[16]  J. Raven,et al.  Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales , 1962 .

[17]  A. Ortmann,et al.  The BCD of response time analysis in experimental economics , 2016, Experimental Economics.

[18]  Daniel J. Acheson,et al.  Increased Alpha-Band Power during the Retention of Shapes and Shape-Location Associations in Visual Short-Term Memory , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[19]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuition and Moral Decision-Making – The Effect of Time Pressure and Cognitive Load on Moral Judgment and Altruistic Behavior , 2016, PloS one.

[20]  Cary Deck,et al.  A Flexible and Customizable Method for Assessing Cognitive Abilities , 2018, Review of Behavioral Economics.

[21]  Jeffrey Paul Carpenter,et al.  Cognitive ability and strategic sophistication , 2013, Games Econ. Behav..

[22]  Sean Duffy,et al.  Cognitive Load in the Multi-Player Prisoner's Dilemma Game: Are There Brains in Games? , 2011 .

[23]  N. Srinivasan,et al.  Registered Replication Report: Rand, Greene, and Nowak (2012) , 2017, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[24]  U. Fischbacher z-Tree: Zurich toolbox for ready-made economic experiments , 1999 .

[25]  F. Zimmerman,et al.  The effects of food advertising and cognitive load on food choices , 2014, BMC Public Health.

[26]  Daniel Västfjäll,et al.  Intuition and cooperation reconsidered , 2013, Nature.

[27]  D. Ariely,et al.  Arousal and economic decision making , 2017 .

[28]  L. Warlop,et al.  Whatever People Say I Am That's What I Am: Social Labeling as a Social Marketing Tool , 2007 .

[29]  Michael D. Robinson,et al.  Boys Don't Cry: Cognitive Load and Priming Increase Stereotypic Sex Differences in Emotion Memory , 2012 .

[30]  Caterina Primi,et al.  The development and testing of a new version of the cognitive reflection test applying item response theory (irt). , 2016 .

[31]  Rick P. Thomas,et al.  Implications of Cognitive Load for Hypothesis Generation and Probability Judgment , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[32]  Laura E. Matzen,et al.  Recreating Raven’s: Software for systematically generating large numbers of Raven-like matrix problems with normed properties , 2010, Behavior research methods.

[33]  Jesse M. Shapiro,et al.  WHO IS 'BEHAVIORAL'? COGNITIVE ABILITY AND ANOMALOUS PREFERENCES. , 2013, Journal of the European Economic Association.

[34]  S. Frederick Journal of Economic Perspectives—Volume 19, Number 4—Fall 2005—Pages 25–42 Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making , 2022 .

[35]  Wim De Neys,et al.  Dual Processing in Reasoning , 2006, Psychological science.

[36]  Roger Ratcliff,et al.  A Theory of Memory Retrieval. , 1978 .

[37]  Nicola Gennaioli,et al.  Competition for Attention , 2013 .

[38]  W. Kirchner Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. , 1958, Journal of experimental psychology.

[39]  Verena Utikal,et al.  Affect and fairness: Dictator games under cognitive load , 2014 .

[40]  David G. Rand,et al.  Time Pressure Increases Cooperation in Competitively Framed Social Dilemmas , 2014, PloS one.

[41]  Lars-Olof Johansson,et al.  Keeping others in our mind or in our heart? Distribution games under cognitive load , 2016 .

[42]  David G. Rand,et al.  Spontaneous giving and calculated greed , 2012, Nature.

[43]  Jan Fekke Ybema,et al.  On preferences and doing the right thing: Satisfaction with advantageous inequity when cognitive processing is limited , 2006 .

[44]  D. Kahneman Thinking, Fast and Slow , 2011 .

[45]  Sendhil Mullainathan,et al.  Poverty Impedes Cognitive Function , 2013, Science.

[46]  Dent,et al.  Cognitive Load and the Equality Heuristic: A Two-Stage Model of Resource Overconsumption in Small Groups. , 2000, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[47]  Sarah R. Allred,et al.  Cognitive Load and Strategic Sophistication , 2014 .

[48]  Steffen Huck,et al.  Who knows it is a game? On strategic awareness and cognitive ability , 2016 .

[49]  J. Tyran,et al.  Risk aversion relates to cognitive ability: Preferences or Noise? , 2016 .

[50]  Cary Deck,et al.  The Effect of Cognitive Load on Economic Decision Making , 2015 .