Habituation of P300 from visual stimuli.

The P300 event-related brain potential (ERP) was elicited with visual stimuli using an oddball task in which the subjects made button press responses to discriminate target and standard stimuli. Each stimulus occurred randomly with a 0.50 probability, and 10 trial blocks were presented at 10-min inter-block-intervals (IBIs). P300 amplitude declined reliably across trial blocks, and an interaction between trial block number and electrode site was observed. No habituation effects were found for P300 latency or for N100, P200, or N200 amplitude. The results suggest that P300 amplitude from visual stimuli habituates when short IBIs and a sufficient number of trial blocks are employed. Theoretical implications are discussed with respect to attentional resource allocation and context updating.

[1]  R. Klein Attention and visual dominance: a chronometric analysis. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  E. N. Sokolov Brain Functions: Neuronal Mechanisms of Learning and Memory , 1977 .

[4]  A. D. Fisk,et al.  The effects of practice and task structure on components of the event-related brain potential. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[5]  J. Cohen,et al.  On the number of trials needed for P300. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[6]  P. Goldman-Rakic,et al.  Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  J. Stein Progress in clinical neurophysiology Vol. 4. Cerebral motor control in man: Long loop mechanisms. Edited by J. E. Desmedt. S. Karger, Basel, 1979, 394 pp. $70.75 , 1980, Neuropsychologia.

[8]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  The effect of stimulus deviation on P3 waves to easily recognized stimuli , 1978, Neuropsychologia.

[9]  S. Shappell,et al.  No decrement in visual P300 amplitude during extended performance of the oddball task. , 1986, The International journal of neuroscience.

[10]  J. Polich,et al.  Comparison of auditory P300 habituation from active and passive conditions. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[11]  E. Donchin,et al.  Psychophysiology : systems, processes, and applications , 1987 .

[12]  D. Siddle,et al.  Orienting and habituation : perspectives in human research , 1983 .

[13]  M. Posner,et al.  Visual dominance: an information-processing account of its origins and significance. , 1976, Psychological review.

[14]  D. Siddle,et al.  Orienting, habituation, and resource allocation: an associative analysis. , 1991, Psychophysiology.

[15]  E. Donchin,et al.  Cognitive Psychophysiology and Human Information Processing , 1986 .

[16]  M. Raichle,et al.  Localization of a human system for sustained attention by positron emission tomography , 1991, Nature.

[17]  J. Polich,et al.  Comparison of P300 from a passive tone sequence paradigm and an active discrimination task. , 1987, Psychophysiology.

[18]  Gavin S. Lew,et al.  P300, habituation, and response mode , 1993, Physiology & Behavior.

[19]  J. Polich,et al.  P300 habituation from visual stimuli? , 1994, Physiology and Behavior.

[20]  M. Verbaten,et al.  Different effects of uncertainty and complexity on single trial visual ERPs and the SCR-OR in non-signal conditions. , 1986, Psychophysiology.

[21]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Varieties of attention , 1984 .

[22]  E. Donchin,et al.  Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[23]  E. Jodo,et al.  Effects of practice on the P300 in a Go/NoGo task. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[24]  J. L. Kenemans,et al.  Visual stimulus change and the orienting reaction: event-related potential evidence for a two-stage process , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[25]  John Polich,et al.  P3(00) habituation from auditory and visual stimuli , 1996, Physiology & Behavior.

[26]  P. Badia,et al.  Habituation of P300 to target stimuli , 1989, Physiology & Behavior.

[27]  Michael I. Posner,et al.  Attention as a Cognitive and Neural System , 1992 .

[28]  W. Roth,et al.  Auditory evoked responses to unpredictable stimuli. , 1973, Psychophysiology.

[29]  Pietro Badia,et al.  Time of day, repeated testing, and interblock interval effects on P300 amplitude , 1990, Physiology & Behavior.

[30]  A. Papanicolaou,et al.  Habituation of auditory event-related potentials: a comparison of self-initiated and automated stimulus trains. , 1987, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[31]  Marinus N. Verbaten,et al.  Time effects on event-related brain potentials and vigilance performance , 1992, Biological Psychology.

[32]  M. Verbaten,et al.  Habituation of early and late visual ERP components and the orienting reaction: the effect of stimulus information. , 1986, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[33]  J. Polich Habituation of P300 from auditory stimuli , 1989, Psychobiology.