A theory of the brain: localist representation is used widely in the brain

ON LOCALIST AND DISTRIBUTED REPRESENTATIONS In this article, I present the theory that localist representation is used widely in the brain starting from its earliest levels of processing. Page (2000) argued for localist representation and Bowers (2009) claimed that the brain uses grandmother cells to code for objects and concepts. However, neither Page (2000) nor Bowers (2009) claimed widespread use of localist representation in the brain. So this is a stronger position than that taken by either. To support the proposed theory, I present neurophysiological evidence, both old and new, and an analysis of localist and distributed representation definitions and models. “Meaning and interpretation” on a stand-alone basis is the fundamental character of a localist unit. In arguing for the proposed theory, I bring to the forefront the “meaning and interpretation” aspect of localist cells and the evidence for it in the brain. I also show that localist and distributed models are not different structurally. In fact, any kind of model can be built with localist units. However, localist representation has no claim on the resulting properties of such models or what they can do.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[2]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: part 1.: an account of basic findings , 1988 .

[3]  T. Gelder,et al.  Mind as Motion: Explorations in the Dynamics of Cognition , 1995 .

[4]  Ira Kemelmacher-Shlizerman,et al.  Being John Malkovich , 2010, ECCV.

[5]  C. Koch,et al.  Category-specific visual responses of single neurons in the human medial temporal lobe , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[7]  C. Koch,et al.  Explicit Encoding of Multimodal Percepts by Single Neurons in the Human Brain , 2009, Current Biology.

[8]  Sowmya Venkataramani,et al.  Orientation Selectivity in Rabbit Retinal Ganglion Cells Is Mediated by Presynaptic Inhibition , 2010, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[9]  R. Shapley,et al.  Cone inputs in macaque primary visual cortex. , 2004, Journal of Neurophysiology.

[10]  S. Thorpe Localized versus distributed representations , 1998 .

[11]  Christof Koch,et al.  On-line, voluntary control of human temporal lobe neurons , 2010, Nature.

[12]  Robert Shapley,et al.  Receptive field structure of neurons in monkey primary visual cortex revealed by stimulation with natural image sequences. , 2002, Journal of vision.

[13]  M. V. Van Hulle,et al.  Encoding of Categories by Noncategory-Specific Neurons in the Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[14]  Emilio Kropff,et al.  Place cells, grid cells, and the brain's spatial representation system. , 2008, Annual review of neuroscience.

[15]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Distributed Representations , 1986, The Philosophy of Artificial Intelligence.

[16]  Moshe Gur,et al.  Cerebral Cortex doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi003 Orientation and Direction Selectivity of Neurons in V1 of Alert Monkeys: Functional Relationships and Laminar Distributions , 2022 .

[17]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Parallel distributed processing: explorations in the microstructure of cognition, vol. 1: foundations , 1986 .

[18]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Locating object knowledge in the brain: comment on Bowers's (2009) attempt to revive the grandmother cell hypothesis. , 2010, Psychological review.

[19]  D. Ringach Mapping receptive fields in primary visual cortex , 2004, The Journal of physiology.

[20]  W. Levick Receptive fields and trigger features of ganglion cells in the visual streak of the rabbit's retina , 1967, The Journal of physiology.

[21]  R. Shapley,et al.  The Orientation Selectivity of Color-Responsive Neurons in Macaque V1 , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  C. Gross Brain, Vision, Memory: Tales in the History of Neuroscience , 1998 .

[23]  Arne D. Ekstrom,et al.  Cellular networks underlying human spatial navigation , 2003, Nature.

[24]  M. Page,et al.  Connectionist modelling in psychology: A localist manifesto , 2000, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[25]  G. Kreiman,et al.  Measuring sparseness in the brain: comment on Bowers (2009). , 2010, Psychological review.

[26]  Charles L. Wilson,et al.  Single Neuron Activity in Human Hippocampus and Amygdala during Recognition of Faces and Objects , 1997, Neuron.

[27]  Jeffrey L. Elman,et al.  Language as a dynamical system , 1996 .

[28]  J. O'Keefe,et al.  The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. , 1971, Brain research.

[29]  C. Koch,et al.  Sparse but not ‘Grandmother-cell’ coding in the medial temporal lobe , 2008, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[30]  W. Usrey,et al.  Receptive fields and response properties of neurons in layer 4 of ferret visual cortex. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  R. Shapley,et al.  The spatial transformation of color in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.

[32]  S. Bloomfield Orientation-sensitive amacrine and ganglion cells in the rabbit retina. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[33]  Jeffrey S Bowers,et al.  On the biological plausibility of grandmother cells: implications for neural network theories in psychology and neuroscience. , 2009, Psychological review.

[34]  R. Reid,et al.  Receptive field structure varies with layer in the primary visual cortex , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.