The Colour of Os: Naturally Biased Associations between Shape and Colour

Many letters of the alphabet are consistently mapped to specific colours by English-speaking adults, both in the general population and in individuals with grapheme – colour synaesthesia who perceive letters in colour. Such associations may be naturally biased by intrinsic sensory cortical organisation, or may be based in literacy (eg ‘A’ is for ‘apple’, apples are red; therefore A is red). To distinguish these two hypotheses, we tested pre-literate children in three experiments and compared their results to those of literate children (aged 7–9 years) and adults. The results indicate that some colour–letter mappings (O white, X black) are naturally biased by the shape of the letter, whereas others (A red, G green) may be based in literacy. They suggest that sensory cortical organisation initially binds colour to some shapes, and that learning to read can induce additional associations, likely through the influence of higher-order networks as letters take on meaning.

[1]  L. Marks On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. , 1975, Psychological bulletin.

[2]  Daphne Maurer,et al.  Do small white balls squeak? Pitch-object correspondences in young children , 2004, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Contour, color and shape analysis beyond the striate cortex , 1985, Vision Research.

[4]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Sound-Colour Synaesthesia: to What Extent Does it Use Cross-Modal Mechanisms Common to us All? , 2006, Cortex.

[5]  Arno Villringer,et al.  Visual Feature and Conjunction Searches of Equal Difficulty Engage Only Partially Overlapping Frontoparietal Networks , 2002, NeuroImage.

[6]  Geoffrey M. Boynton,et al.  Individual Differences among Grapheme-Color Synesthetes: Brain-Behavior Correlations , 2005, Neuron.

[7]  Martin S. Lindauer The meanings of the physiognomic stimuli taketa and maluma , 1990 .

[8]  Richard B. Ivry,et al.  Coming Unbound: Disrupting Automatic Integration of Synesthetic Color and Graphemes by Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation of the Right Parietal Lobe , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[9]  M. Elliott,et al.  Flicker-induced color and form: Interdependencies and relation to stimulation frequency and phase , 2006, Consciousness and Cognition.

[10]  Lawrence E. Marks,et al.  On colored-hearing synesthesia: cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. , 1975 .

[11]  W. Singer,et al.  Neuronal Correlates of Colour-Graphemic Synaesthesia: Afmri Study , 2006, Cortex.

[12]  C. Westbury Implicit sound symbolism in lexical access: Evidence from an interference task , 2005, Brain and Language.

[13]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Non-random associations of graphemes to colours in synaesthetic and non-synaesthetic populations , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[14]  Jamie Ward,et al.  Synaesthesia: The Prevalence of Atypical Cross-Modal Experiences , 2006, Perception.

[15]  D. Maurer,et al.  The shape of boubas: sound-shape correspondences in toddlers and adults. , 2006, Developmental science.

[16]  Vincent Walsh,et al.  Synaesthesia: supernormal integration? , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  V. Ramachandran,et al.  Synaesthesia? A window into perception, thought and language , 2001 .

[18]  Anina N. Rich,et al.  A systematic, large-scale study of synaesthesia: implications for the role of early experience in lexical-colour associations , 2005, Cognition.

[19]  Lawrence E. Marks,et al.  On Perceptual Metaphors , 1996 .

[20]  J. Schramme,et al.  100 Years of Benham's Top in Colour Science , 1995, Perception.