Reaching for the Unreachable: Reorganization of Reaching with Walking

Previous research suggests that reaching and walking behaviors may be linked developmentally as reaching changes at the onset of walking. Here we report new evidence on an apparent loss of the distinction between the reachable and nonreachable distances as children start walking. The experiment compared nonwalkers, walkers with help, and independent walkers in a reaching task to targets at varying distances. Reaching attempts, contact, leaning, and communication behaviors were recorded. Most of the children reached for the unreachable objects the first time it was presented. Nonwalkers, however, reached less on the subsequent trials showing clear adjustment of their reaching decisions with the failures. On the contrary, walkers consistently attempted reaches to targets at unreachable distances. We suggest that these reaching errors may result from inappropriate integration of reaching and locomotor actions, attention control and near/far visual space. We propose a reward-mediated model implemented on a NAO humanoid robot that replicates the main results from our study showing an increase in reaching attempts to nonreachable distances after the onset of walking.

[1]  J. Piaget The construction of reality in the child , 1954 .

[2]  J. Field The adjustment of reaching behavior to object distance in early infancy. , 1976, Child development.

[3]  J. Field Relation of Young Infants' Reaching Behavior to Stimulus Distance and Solidity. , 1976 .

[4]  C. Rovee-Collier,et al.  Advances in infancy research , 1981 .

[5]  M. Lamb,et al.  Infant social cognition : empirical and theoretical considerations , 1981 .

[6]  E. Thelen Development of Coordinated Movement: Implications for Early Human Development , 1986 .

[7]  B. Bertenthal,et al.  A systems approach to the organizing effects of self-produced locomotion during infancy , 1990 .

[8]  R. K. Clifton,et al.  Infants' Perception of Auditory Space. , 1991 .

[9]  P. Rochat,et al.  Object representation guides infants' reaching in the dark. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[10]  E. Thelen,et al.  Hidden skills: a dynamic systems analysis of treadmill stepping during the first year. , 1991, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development.

[11]  N. Fox,et al.  The relations between frontal brain electrical activity and cognitive development during infancy. , 1992, Child development.

[12]  A. Yonas,et al.  Effective action by infants to contact objects by reaching and leaning. , 1993, Child development.

[13]  A. Yonas,et al.  Perceiving the affordance of contact in four- and five-month-old infants. , 1993, Child development.

[14]  P. Rochat,et al.  Development of sitting and reaching in 5- to 6-month-old infants☆ , 1995 .

[15]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  A dynamic systems approach to the development of cognition and action. , 2007, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.

[16]  M. Appelbaum,et al.  Affective reorganization in the infant, the mother, and the dyad: the role of upright locomotion and its timing. , 1995, Child development.

[17]  P. Rochat,et al.  An account of the systematic error in judging what is reachable. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[18]  A. Berthoz,et al.  Reaching beyond reach , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[19]  M. Hadders‐Algra,et al.  The development of postural adjustments during reaching in 6- to 18-month-old infants Evidence for two transitions , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.

[20]  P. Rochat,et al.  To reach or not to reach? Perception of body effectivities by young infants , 1999 .

[21]  David I. Anderson,et al.  Travel Broadens the Mind. , 2000, Infancy : the official journal of the International Society on Infant Studies.

[22]  A. Berti,et al.  When Far Becomes Near: Remapping of Space by Tool Use , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[23]  Janette Atkinson,et al.  The Developing Visual Brain , 2000 .

[24]  K. Adolph Specificity of Learning: Why Infants Fall Over a Veritable Cliff , 2000, Psychological science.

[25]  M. H. Fischer,et al.  Estimating reachability: Whole body engagement or postural stability? , 2000 .

[26]  Nicola Smania,et al.  Coding of Far and Near Space in Neglect Patients , 2001, NeuroImage.

[27]  D. Corbetta,et al.  Infants Return to Two-Handed Reaching When They Are Learning to Walk , 2002, Journal of motor behavior.

[28]  M. Anthony Lewis,et al.  Strategies and determinants for selection of alternate foot placement during human locomotion: influence of spatial and temporal constraints , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[29]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[30]  H. Hayne,et al.  Amount of Crawling and Walking Experience has Varying Effects on Cognitive Development During Infancy , 2006 .

[31]  T. Higuchi,et al.  Action‐oriented representation of peripersonal and extrapersonal space: Insights from manual and locomotor actions1 , 2006 .

[32]  J. Triesch,et al.  Emergence of Disparity Tuning during the Development of Vergence Eye Movements , 2007, 2007 IEEE 6th International Conference on Development and Learning.

[33]  Jane E. Clark,et al.  Two steps forward and one back: Learning to walk affects infants' sitting posture. , 2007, Infant behavior & development.

[34]  J. Campos,et al.  Development of Autonomy: Role of Walking Onset and its Timing , 2008, Perceptual and motor skills.

[35]  Minoru Asada,et al.  Between Frustration and Elation: Sense of Control Regulates the lntrinsic Motivation for Motor Learning , 2011, Lifelong Learning.

[36]  Thomas H. Weisswange,et al.  Bayesian Cue Integration as a Developmental Outcome of Reward Mediated Learning , 2011, PloS one.

[37]  Marco Antonelli,et al.  Implicit Sensorimotor Mapping of the Peripersonal Space by Gazing and Reaching , 2011, IEEE Transactions on Autonomous Mental Development.

[38]  Britta Wrede,et al.  Reward-based learning of optimal cue integration in audio and visual depth estimation , 2011, 2011 15th International Conference on Advanced Robotics (ICAR).

[39]  M. Clearfield Learning to walk changes infants' social interactions. , 2011, Infant behavior & development.

[40]  G. M,et al.  Motor Development in Children : Aspects of Coordination and Control , 2011 .

[41]  Marco Antonelli,et al.  Plastic Representation of the Reachable Space for a Humanoid Robot , 2012, SAB.

[42]  Linda B. Smith,et al.  How Walking Influences the Development of Absolute Distance Perception , 2012, SAB.