The location but not the attributes of visual cues are automatically encoded into working memory

Although it has been well known that visual cues affect the perception of subsequent visual stimuli, relatively little is known about how the cues themselves are processed. The present study attempted to characterize the processing of a visual cue by investigating what information about the cue is stored in terms of both location ("where" is the cue) and attributes ("what" are the attributes of the cue). In 11 experiments subjects performed several trials of reporting a target letter and then answered an unexpected question about the cue (e.g., the location, color, or identity of the cue). This surprise question revealed that participants could report the location of the cue even when the cue never indicated the target location and they were explicitly told to ignore it. Furthermore, the memory trace of this location information endured during encoding of the subsequent target. In contrast to location, attributes of the cue (e.g., color) were poorly reported, even for attributes that were used by subjects to perform the task. These results shed new light on the mechanisms underlying cueing effects and suggest also that the visual system may create empty object files in response to visual cues.

[1]  D. Kahneman,et al.  The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[2]  Philipp Koehn,et al.  Cognitive Psychology , 1992, Ageing and Society.

[3]  M. Nieuwenstein,et al.  The attentional blink provides episodic distinctiveness: sparing at a cost. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[4]  Vision Research , 1961, Nature.

[5]  R. Marois,et al.  The attentional blink: A review of data and theory , 2009, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[6]  Daniel J. Simons,et al.  Inattentional blindness , 2007, Scholarpedia.

[7]  G. Boynton,et al.  Rapid and reflexive feature-based attention. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[8]  S. Yantis,et al.  Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: voluntary versus automatic allocation. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[9]  L. Maloney,et al.  Planning multiple movements within a fixed time limit: the cost of constrained time allocation in a visuo-motor task. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[10]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Attention alters appearance , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  G. Alvarez,et al.  Space and time, not surface features, guide object persistence , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[12]  N. Kanwisher,et al.  Objects, Attributes, and Visual Attention: Which, What, and Where , 1992 .

[13]  Liqiang Huang,et al.  Characterizing the Nature of Visual Conscious Access: The Distinction between Features and Locations , 2010, Journal of vision.

[14]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[15]  B. Wyble,et al.  The attentional blink: Past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness , 2010, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  Julie D. Golomb,et al.  The influence of object location on identity: a "spatial congruency bias". , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[17]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[18]  H. Pashler,et al.  Negligible Effect of Spatial Precuing on Identification of Single Digits , 1994 .

[19]  Hui Chen,et al.  Amnesia for Object Attributes , 2015, Psychological science.

[20]  Y. Yeshurun,et al.  Blinded by irrelevance: pure irrelevance induced "blindness". , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[21]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[22]  Paul M Bays,et al.  Temporal dynamics of encoding, storage, and reallocation of visual working memory. , 2011, Journal of vision.

[23]  I. Rock,et al.  Perception without attention: Results of a new method , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.

[24]  V. Lollo,et al.  Temporal characteristics of iconic memory , 1977, Nature.

[25]  Brad Wyble,et al.  Attentional episodes in visual perception. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[26]  B. Wyble,et al.  The simultaneous type, serial token model of temporal attention and working memory. , 2007, Psychological review.

[27]  B. Scholl,et al.  Divide and Conquer , 2004, Psychology Science.

[28]  J. Wolfe Inattentional Amnesia , 2000 .

[29]  H. Egeth,et al.  Perception without attention: evidence of grouping under conditions of inattention. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Distortions in recall from visual memory: two classes of attractors at work. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[31]  V. Coltheart Fleeting memories : cognition of brief visual stimuli , 1999 .

[32]  M. Cheal,et al.  Attention Effects on Form Discrimination at Different Eccentricities , 1989, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[33]  B. Scholl,et al.  The relationship between object files and conscious perception , 2005, Cognition.

[34]  M. Posner,et al.  Orienting of Attention* , 1980, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.