Forecasting and Backcasting: Predicting the Impact of Events on the Future

In many choices they make—for example, choosing between a movie and a play or deciding whether to attend a sports game shortly before a birthday party—consumers are guided by how they expect an event will make them feel. They may predict their feelings by forecasting (imagining their feelings when the impacting event occurs, then considering how those feelings might change over time) or by backcasting (imagining their feelings in a future period, then considering how those feelings might be different were the impacting event to happen). Four studies show that backcasters expect events to have a greater hedonic impact than do forecasters, largely because they think more about the impacting event. The studies also reveal that backcasters consider other information that forecasters tend to ignore.

[1]  Zeynep Gürhan-Canli,et al.  The Effect of Expected Variability of Product Quality and Attribute Uniqueness on Family Brand Evaluations , 2003 .

[2]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Experiences extended across time: Evaluation of moments and episodes , 1992 .

[3]  P. Herr,et al.  Order Effects in Consumer Judgment, Choice, and Memory: the Role of Initial Processing Goals , 1990 .

[4]  B. K. Buyukkurt,et al.  Integration of Serially Sampled Price Information: Modeling and Some Findings , 1986 .

[5]  G. W. Fischer,et al.  Preferences for separating or combining events. , 1991, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[6]  J. Leyens,et al.  Intergroup Relations in Soccer Finals: People's Forecasts of the Duration of Emotional Reactions of In-Group and Out-Group Soccer Fans , 2005, The Journal of social psychology.

[7]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  The pleasures of uncertainty: prolonging positive moods in ways people do not anticipate. , 2005, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[8]  D. Kahneman,et al.  Heuristics and Biases: The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment , 2002 .

[9]  I. Simonson,et al.  THE INFLUENCE OF ANTICIPATING REGRET AND RESPONSIBILITY ON PURCHASE DECISIONS , 1992 .

[10]  C. Finkenauer,et al.  Investigating the Role of Time in Affective Forecasting: Temporal Influences on Forecasting Accuracy , 2007, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[11]  Joel Huber,et al.  The Impact of Anticipating Satisfaction on Consumer Choice , 2000 .

[12]  R. Morris,et al.  The Cognitive Psychology of Planning , 2004 .

[13]  Stephen J. Gould,et al.  An interpretive study of purposeful, mood self-regulating consumption: The consumption and mood framework , 1997 .

[14]  J. Gross Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[15]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Affective Forecasting , 2005 .

[16]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Prospection: Experiencing the Future , 2007, Science.

[17]  John M. Neale,et al.  Prospective and cross-sectional mood reports offer no evidence of a "blue Monday" phenomenon. , 1985 .

[18]  Curtis P. Haugtvedt,et al.  Message Order Effects in Persuasion: An Attitude Strength Perspective , 1994 .

[19]  D. Wegner,et al.  On being cool and collected: mood regulation in anticipation of social interaction. , 1996, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[20]  B. Mellers Choice and the relative pleasure of consequences. , 2000, Psychological bulletin.

[21]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Loss Aversion Is an Affective Forecasting Error , 2006, Psychological science.

[22]  Hamid Noori,et al.  Developing the "right" breakthrough product/service: an umbrella methodology - Part A , 1999 .

[23]  Carey K. Morewedge,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article The Peculiar Longevity of Things Not So Bad , 2022 .

[24]  Looking Through the Crystal Ball , 2006 .

[25]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  The Future Is Now: Temporal Correction in Affective Forecasting☆ , 2002 .

[26]  George Loewenstein,et al.  Predicting and indulging changing preferences. , 2003 .

[27]  Gita Venkataramani Johar,et al.  A Varying-Parameter Averaging Model of On-Line Brand Evaluations , 1997 .

[28]  Tammo H. A. Bijmolt,et al.  Consumer Memory for Television Advertising: A Field Study of Duration, Serial Position, and Competition Effects , 1997 .

[29]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Location, Location, Location: The Misprediction of Satisfaction in Housing Lotteries , 2003, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[30]  Brian J. Gibbs,et al.  Intuitive Hedonics: Consumer Beliefs About the Dynamics of Liking , 1995 .

[31]  Timothy D. Wilson,et al.  Immune neglect: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. , 1998, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[32]  Lawrence A. Crosby,et al.  Effects of Consumer Information and Education on Cognition and Choice , 1981 .

[33]  R. Buehler,et al.  Cultural Differences in Affective Forecasting: The Role of Focalism , 2005, Personality & social psychology bulletin.

[34]  N. Epley,et al.  Putting Adjustment Back in the Anchoring and Adjustment Heuristic: Differential Processing of Self-Generated and Experimenter-Provided Anchors , 2001, Psychological science.

[35]  D. Read,et al.  Predicting Hunger: The Effects of Appetite and Delay on Choice. , 1998, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[36]  M. Zeelenberg,et al.  Consequences of Regret Aversion: Effects of Expected Feedback on Risky Decision Making , 1996 .

[37]  E. Igou Lay theories in affective forecasting: The progression of affect , 2004 .

[38]  C. W. Park,et al.  Not as Happy as I Thought I'd Be? Affective Misforecasting and Product Evaluations , 2007 .

[39]  Daniel Kahneman,et al.  Predicting a changing taste: Do people know what they will like? , 1992 .

[40]  C. W. Park,et al.  Emerald Book Chapter : Looking Through the Crystal Ball : Affective Forecasting and Misforecasting in Consumer Behavior , 2022 .

[41]  B. Mellers,et al.  Anticipated Emotions as Guides to Choice , 2001 .

[42]  D. Gilbert,et al.  Focalism: a source of durability bias in affective forecasting. , 2000, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[43]  R. Buehler,et al.  Intensity Bias in Affective Forecasting: The Role of Temporal Focus , 2001 .

[44]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty: Heuristics and Biases , 1974, Science.

[45]  J. Bryant,et al.  Temporal Changes in Mood Repair Through Music Consumption: Effects of Mood, Mood Salience, and Individual Differences , 2007 .

[46]  B. Tabachnick,et al.  Using Multivariate Statistics , 1983 .

[47]  D. Campbell,et al.  Recency and primacy in persuasion as a function of the timing of speeches and measurements. , 1959, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[48]  John Holmberg,et al.  Backcasting — a framework for strategic planning , 2000 .

[49]  D. Gilbert Heuristics and Biases: Inferential Correction , 2002 .

[50]  A. Tversky Features of Similarity , 1977 .

[51]  Amos Tversky,et al.  Studies of similarity , 1978 .

[52]  A. Tversky,et al.  Judgment under Uncertainty , 1982 .

[53]  C. I. Hovland The Order Of Presentation In Persuasion , 1966 .

[54]  T. Connolly,et al.  Predicting Affective Responses to Unexpected Outcomes. , 2001, Organizational behavior and human decision processes.

[55]  D. Dunning,et al.  Mental addition versus subtraction in counterfactual reasoning: on assessing the impact of personal actions and life events. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[56]  J. van der Pligt,et al.  Anticipated Affect and Behavioral Choice , 1996 .