Medial prefrontal dissociations during processing of trait diagnostic and nondiagnostic person information.

Previous research has suggested that perceivers spontaneously extract trait-specific information from the behaviour of others. However, little is known about whether perceivers spontaneously engage in the same depth of social-cognitive processing for all person information or reserve such processing specifically for information that conveys diagnostic clues about another person's dispositions. Moreover, a question remains as to whether the processing of such nondiagnostic information can be affected by perceivers' explicit goal to consider another's dispositions or not. To examine processing of diagnostic and nondiagnostic social information as a function of perceivers' explicit social-cognitive goals, participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scanning while performing social (impression formation) or non-social orienting tasks using statements that conveyed either diagnostic or nondiagnostic information about the target's personality traits. Replicating two earlier studies, results identified a region of dorsal medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) that was preferentially activated by impression formation. Interestingly, no difference between trait-diagnostic and nondiagnostic information was observed when participants had the explicit goal of forming an impression, but a substantial effect of diagnosticity emerged when task instructions oriented them away from considering the target as a social agent. These results suggest that trait-nondiagnostic information is not subject to spontaneous social-cognitive processing, but that such processing may nevertheless occur when perceivers have the explicit goal to use that information to form an impression of a target.

[1]  Mahzarin R. Banaji,et al.  Encoding-Specific Effects of Social Cognition on the Neural Correlates of Subsequent Memory , 2004, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[3]  G. Shulman,et al.  Medial prefrontal cortex and self-referential mental activity: Relation to a default mode of brain function , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[4]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[5]  Von O. Leirer,et al.  Cognitive representation of personality impressions: Organizational processes in first impression formation. , 1980 .

[6]  R. Saxe,et al.  Making sense of another mind: The role of the right temporo-parietal junction , 2005, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  Robert S. Wyer,et al.  The cognitive representation of persons and groups and its effect on recall and recognition memory , 1984 .

[8]  J. Uleman,et al.  How automatic are social judgments? , 1985, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[9]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[10]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[11]  C. Frith,et al.  Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’ , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[12]  T. K. Srull,et al.  Person memory and judgment. , 1989, Psychological review.

[13]  M. Raichle,et al.  Searching for a baseline: Functional imaging and the resting human brain , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Social cognitive neuroscience: where are we heading? , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[15]  Alexander Todorov,et al.  Attributions on the brain: Neuro-imaging dispositional inferences, beyond theory of mind , 2005, NeuroImage.

[16]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  The Link between Social Cognition and Self-referential Thought in the Medial Prefrontal Cortex , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[17]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  Forming impressions of people versus inanimate objects: Social-cognitive processing in the medial prefrontal cortex , 2005, NeuroImage.

[18]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Distinct neural systems subserve person and object knowledge , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[19]  R. Hastie,et al.  Person memory: Personality traits as organizing principles in memory for behaviors. , 1979 .

[20]  M. Hallett,et al.  Modeling other minds , 1995, Neuroreport.

[21]  Alexander Todorov,et al.  The efficiency of binding spontaneous trait inferences to actors’ faces , 2003 .

[22]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  D. Hamilton,et al.  Cognitive organization of impressions: effects of incongruency in complex representations. , 1989, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[24]  Matthew Flatt,et al.  PsyScope: An interactive graphic system for designing and controlling experiments in the psychology laboratory using Macintosh computers , 1993 .

[25]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[26]  Gordon B. Moskowitz,et al.  People as Flexible Interpreters: Evidence and Issues from Spontaneous Trait Inference , 1996 .

[27]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[28]  J. Uleman,et al.  Unintended effects of goals on unintended inferences. , 1994, Journal of personality and social psychology.

[29]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.