Parametric induction of animacy experience
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] T. Allison,et al. Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[2] K. Hiraki,et al. An event-related potentials study of biological motion perception in human infants. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[3] U. Frith. Emanuel Miller lecture: confusions and controversies about Asperger syndrome. , 2004, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.
[4] G Johansson,et al. Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception , 1976, Psychological research.
[5] G. Csibra. Goal attribution to inanimate agents by 6.5-month-old infants , 2008, Cognition.
[6] S. Lea,et al. Visual Perception of Intentional Motion , 1994, Perception.
[7] C. Frith,et al. Schizophrenia and theory of mind , 2004, Psychological Medicine.
[8] C. Frith,et al. Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.
[9] A. Premack,et al. Causal cognition : a multidisciplinary debate , 1996 .
[10] Gordon Claridge,et al. Schizotypy: Implications for Illness and Health , 2002 .
[11] John E. Opfer,et al. Identifying living and sentient kinds from dynamic information: the case of goal-directed versus aimless autonomous movement in conceptual change , 2002, Cognition.
[12] Max Coltheart,et al. Mentalising, schizotypy, and schizophrenia , 1999, Cognition.
[13] Life is not just in the fast lane: dissociating the perceptions of speed and animacy , 2007 .
[14] E. Gyulai,et al. Considerations on Perception of “Animacy” in the Motion of a Single Object , 2004, Perceptual and motor skills.
[15] R. Bentall,et al. Persecutory delusions: a review and theoretical integration. , 2001, Clinical psychology review.
[16] Z. Nadasdy,et al. Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age , 1995, Cognition.
[17] G. Csibra,et al. Goal attribution without agency cues: the perception of ‘pure reason’ in infancy , 1999, Cognition.
[18] Saul Sternberg,et al. The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .
[19] J. Gibson. The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1979 .
[20] J. Stewart. PERCEPTION OF ANIMACY , 1982 .
[21] Csaba Veres,et al. The perceived intentionality of groups , 1999, Cognition.
[22] B. Rimé,et al. The perception of interpersonal emotions originated by patterns of movement , 1985 .
[23] Uta Frith,et al. Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.
[24] L. Kaufman,et al. Distinguishing Between Animates And Inanimates: Not By Motion Alone , 1995 .
[25] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[26] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. The influence of spatial context and the role of intentionality in the interpretation of animacy from motion , 2006, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] D. Premack,et al. The perception of intention. , 1989, Science.
[28] Pascal Boyer,et al. How the brain perceives causality: an event-related fMRI study , 2001, Neuroreport.
[29] J. N. Bassili. Temporal and spatial contingencies in the perception of social events , 1976 .
[30] J. Burns. The social brain hypothesis of schizophrenia. , 2006, World psychiatry : official journal of the World Psychiatric Association.
[31] A. Schlottmann,et al. Do 9-Month-Olds Perceive Causation-at-a-Distance? , 1999, Perception.
[32] Carey K. Morewedge,et al. Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. , 2007, Journal of personality and social psychology.
[33] Nicole C. Krämer,et al. Computer Animated Movement and Person Perception: Methodological Advances in Nonverbal Behavior Research , 2001 .
[34] Raymond A Mar,et al. Triggering the intentional stance. , 2007, Novartis Foundation symposium.
[35] G. Csibra,et al. Teleological reasoning in infancy: the naı̈ve theory of rational action , 2003, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[36] Stefanie Hoehl,et al. The perception of biological motion by infants: An event-related potential study , 2006, Neuroscience Letters.
[37] A. Leslie. Spatiotemporal Continuity and the Perception of Causality in Infants , 1984, Perception.
[38] Nicole C. Krämer,et al. Transcript-based computer animation of movement: Evaluating a new tool for nonverbal behavior research , 2001, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.
[39] Karl J. Friston,et al. PET Imaging and Cognition in Schizophrenia , 1992, Journal of the Royal Society of Medicine.
[40] S. Gelman,et al. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity In Cognition And Culture , 1994 .
[41] R. Blake,et al. Perception of human motion. , 2007, Annual review of psychology.
[42] D. Premack. The infant's theory of self-propelled objects , 1990, Cognition.
[43] Ken Springer,et al. Nonverbal bases of social perception: Developmental change in sensitivity to patterns of motion that reveal interpersonal events , 1996 .
[44] Susan C. Johnson,et al. Inferring the goals of a nonhuman agent , 2001 .
[45] Mark H. Johnson,et al. Eye contact detection in humans from birth , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[46] Stephen J. Misovich,et al. Effects of Disruption of Structure and Motion on Perceptions of Social Causality , 1992 .
[47] R. Baillargeon,et al. Can a Self-Propelled Box Have a Goal? , 2005, Psychological science.
[48] A. Meltzoff,et al. The detection of contingency and animacy from simple animations in the human brain. , 2003, Cerebral cortex.
[49] A. Leslie. Mapping the mind: ToMM, ToBY, and Agency: Core architecture and domain specificity , 1994 .
[50] Robin M. Murray,et al. Cognitive neuropsychiatric models of persecutory delusions. , 2001, The American journal of psychiatry.
[51] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perceptual causality and animacy , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[52] D. Wegner,et al. Dimensions of Mind Perception , 2007, Science.
[53] Edward E. Smith,et al. A Parametric Study of Prefrontal Cortex Involvement in Human Working Memory , 1996, NeuroImage.
[54] Maja J. Matarić,et al. Human Simulation of Adaptive Behavior: Interactive studies of pursuit, evasion, courtship, fighting, and play , 1996 .
[55] C. Boyatzis,et al. Children's facial and gestural decoding and encoding: Relations between skills and with popularity , 1994 .
[56] P. Rochat,et al. Young infants' sensitivity to movement information specifying social causality , 1997 .
[57] N. Emery,et al. The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze , 2000, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.
[58] U. Frith,et al. Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development , 2000 .
[59] S. Baron-Cohen,et al. Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.
[60] T. Brazelton,et al. The infant's response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. , 1978, Journal of the American Academy of Child Psychiatry.
[61] David Premack,et al. Intention as psychological cause , 1996 .
[62] D. Rakison,et al. Developmental origin of the animate-inanimate distinction. , 2001, Psychological bulletin.
[63] P. Todd,et al. Accurate judgments of intention from motion cues alone: A cross-cultural study , 2005 .
[64] F. Heider,et al. An experimental study of apparent behavior , 1944 .
[65] Gary Bente,et al. Person perception effects of computer-simulated male and female head movement , 1996 .
[66] Patrice D. Tremoulet,et al. Perception of Animacy from the Motion of a Single Object , 2000, Perception.
[67] Karl J. Friston,et al. Activation in Posterior Superior Temporal Sulcus Parallels Parameter Inducing the Percept of Animacy , 2005, Neuron.
[68] J. Bruner,et al. The capacity for joint visual attention in the infant , 1975, Nature.