Implicit sequence learning despite multitasking: the role of across-task predictability
暂无分享,去创建一个
Fang Zhao | Eva Röttger | Hilde Haider | Robert Gaschler | H. Haider | R. Gaschler | F. Zhao | Eva Röttger
[1] C. Buss,et al. Children's Brain Development Benefits from Longer Gestation , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[2] R. Aslin,et al. Statistical learning in a serial reaction time task: access to separable statistical cues by individual learners. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[3] D. J. Marcus,et al. Oculomotor evidence of sequence learning on the serial reaction time task , 2006, Memory & cognition.
[4] G. Dreisbach,et al. Conflict and disfluency as aversive signals: context-specific processing adjustments are modulated by affective location associations , 2018, Psychological research.
[5] Pierre Jolicoeur,et al. Testing the predictions of the central capacity sharing model. , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[6] F. Plessow,et al. Successful voluntary recruitment of cognitive control under acute stress , 2017, Cognition.
[7] Wilfried Kunde,et al. Who is talking in backward crosstalk? Disentangling response- from goal-conflict in dual-task performance , 2014, Cognition.
[8] H. Pashler. Dual-task interference in simple tasks: data and theory. , 1994, Psychological bulletin.
[9] Morten H. Christiansen,et al. Statistical learning of probabilistic nonadjacent dependencies by multiple-cue integration , 2012 .
[10] H Heuer,et al. Secondary-task effects on sequence learning , 1996, Psychological research.
[11] Marco Steinhauser,et al. Serial or parallel processing in dual tasks: what is more effortful? , 2009, Psychophysiology.
[12] Dorit Wenke,et al. Implicit sequence learning based on instructed task set. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[13] B. Hommel. Event Files: Evidence for Automatic Integration of Stimulus-Response Episodes , 1998 .
[14] Hilde Haider,et al. Implicit visual learning and the expression of learning , 2013, Consciousness and Cognition.
[15] M. A. Stadler,et al. Role of attention in implicit learning. , 1995 .
[16] H. Haider,et al. An old problem: How can we distinguish between conscious and unconscious knowledge acquired in an implicit learning task? , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.
[17] N. Yeung,et al. The role of prediction and outcomes in adaptive cognitive control , 2015, Journal of Physiology-Paris.
[18] Robert Gaschler,et al. More Than Hitting the Correct Key Quickly. , 2019, Experimental psychology.
[19] R. Remington,et al. The role of input and output modality pairings in dual-task performance: Evidence for content-dependent central interference , 2006, Cognitive Psychology.
[20] R. Ulrich,et al. Processing two tasks with varying task order: central stage duration influences central processing order. , 2011, Acta psychologica.
[21] Victor Mittelstädt,et al. Separating Limits on Preparation Versus Online Processing in Multitasking Paradigms: Evidence for Resource Models , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[22] D. Navon,et al. Queuing or Sharing? A Critical Evaluation of the Single-Bottleneck Notion , 2002, Cognitive Psychology.
[23] E. Hazeltine,et al. Dual-Task Processing With Identical Stimulus and Response Sets: Assessing the Importance of Task Representation in Dual-Task Interference , 2018, Front. Psychol..
[24] Nicolas W. Schuck,et al. Position-item associations play a role in the acquisition of order knowledge in an implicit serial reaction time task. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[25] S. Keele,et al. The cognitive and neural architecture of sequence representation. , 2003, Psychological review.
[26] T. Braver,et al. Dissociating proactive and reactive control in the Stroop task , 2016, Memory & cognition.
[27] James R. Schmidt,et al. Context-specific temporal learning with non-conflict stimuli: proof-of-principle for a learning account of context-specific proportion congruent effects , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[28] H Pashler,et al. Processing stages in overlapping tasks: evidence for a central bottleneck. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[29] Rolf Ulrich,et al. Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: Models and contamination effects , 2008, Cognitive Psychology.
[30] Rolf Ulrich,et al. On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: Effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[31] Markus Janczyk,et al. Two types of backward crosstalk: Sequential modulations and evidence from the diffusion model. , 2019, Acta psychologica.
[32] Karl J. Friston. The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.
[33] R. French,et al. Implicit learning and consciousness: A graded, dynamic perspective , 2002 .
[34] Z. Dienes,et al. Implicit learning: Below the subjective threshold , 1997 .
[35] Wouter Kruijne,et al. Outlines of a multiple trace theory of temporal preparation , 2014, Front. Psychol..
[36] M. Nissen,et al. Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence from performance measures , 1987, Cognitive Psychology.
[37] James R. Schmidt. Temporal Learning and List-Level Proportion Congruency: Conflict Adaptation or Learning When to Respond? , 2013, PloS one.
[38] G. Dreisbach,et al. Conflicts as Aversive Signals for Control Adaptation , 2015 .
[39] H. Haider,et al. Implicit visual learning: How the task set modulates learning by determining the stimulus–response binding , 2014, Consciousness and Cognition.
[40] Hilde Haider,et al. Abstract Feature Codes: The Building Blocks of the Implicit Learning System , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[41] Torsten Schubert,et al. Improved Intertask Coordination after Extensive Dual-Task Practice , 2011, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[42] Mike Wendt,et al. Conflict adaptation in time: Foreperiods as contextual cues for attentional adjustment , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[43] P. Jolicoeur,et al. A central capacity sharing model of dual-task performance. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[44] M. Masson,et al. Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[45] Daniel B. Willingham,et al. Implicit motor sequence learning is represented in response locations , 2000, Memory & cognition.
[46] Wouter Kruijne,et al. Hazard Versus History: Temporal Preparation Is Driven by Past Experience , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[47] Jeff Miller,et al. Backward crosstalk effects in psychological refractory period paradigms: effects of second-task response types on first-task response latencies , 2006, Psychological research.
[48] Asher Cohen,et al. Involuntary strategy-dependent dual task performance , 2011, Psychological research.
[49] Rico Fischer,et al. Efficient multitasking: parallel versus serial processing of multiple tasks , 2015, Front. Psychol..
[50] Dorit Wenke,et al. Time-Based Expectancy in Temporally Structured Task Switching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[51] Ronald Hübner,et al. Strategies of flanker coprocessing in single and dual tasks. , 2007, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[52] E. Hazeltine,et al. Understanding Central Processes: The Case against Simple Stimulus-Response Associations and for Complex Task Representation , 2016 .
[53] R. D. Gordon,et al. Executive control of visual attention in dual-task situations. , 2001, Psychological review.
[54] A. Cohen,et al. Modularity beyond perception: evidence from the PRP paradigm. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[55] H. Haider,et al. How task representations guide attention: further evidence for the shielding function of task sets. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[56] J. C. Johnston,et al. Chronometric Evidence for Central Postponement in Temporally Overlapping Tasks , 2003 .
[57] A. Welford. THE ‘PSYCHOLOGICAL REFRACTORY PERIOD’ AND THE TIMING OF HIGH‐SPEED PERFORMANCE—A REVIEW AND A THEORY , 1952 .
[58] Rico Fischer,et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Learning , Memory , and Cognition Context-Sensitive Adjustment of Cognitive Control in Dual-Task Performance , 2013 .
[59] B. Hommel. Automatic stimulus-response translation in dual-task performance. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[60] P. Frensch,et al. Implicit learning of unique and ambiguous serial transitions in the presence and absence of a distractor task. , 1994 .
[61] Richard I. Ivry,et al. Attention and structure in sequence learning. , 1990 .
[62] Eliot Hazeltine,et al. Simultaneous dual-task performance reveals parallel response selection after practice. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] George Houghton,et al. Parallel Models of Serial Behaviour: Lashley Revisited , 1995 .
[64] Eva Röttger,et al. Why Prediction Matters in Multitasking and How Predictability Can Improve It , 2017, Front. Psychol..
[65] W. Kunde,et al. A common mechanism behind distractor-response and response-effect binding? , 2016, Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics.
[66] Andrea Kiesel,et al. Cognitive Structure, Flexibility, and Plasticity in Human Multitasking—An Integrative Review of Dual-Task and Task-Switching Research , 2018, Psychological bulletin.
[67] Luis Jiménez,et al. Sequential congruency effects in implicit sequence learning , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.
[68] I Koch,et al. Automatic and intentional activation of task sets. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[69] D E Kieras,et al. A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. , 1997, Psychological review.
[70] Antje S. Meyer,et al. EMPIRICAL STUDY Concurrent Statistical Learning of Adjacent and Nonadjacent Dependencies , 2016 .
[71] D. Wentura,et al. Distractor Repetitions Retrieve Previous Responses to Targets , 2007, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.
[72] Luis Jiménez,et al. Representing serial action and perception , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[73] E. Hazeltine,et al. Conceptualization of task boundaries preserves implicit sequence learning under dual-task conditions , 2013, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[74] B. Hommel,et al. What do we learn from binding features? Evidence for multilevel feature integration. , 2006, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[75] Axel Cleeremans. The Radical Plasticity Thesis: How the Brain Learns to be Conscious , 2011, Front. Psychology.
[76] P. Frensch,et al. The influence of task instruction on action coding: constraint setting or direct coding? , 2005, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[77] Iring Koch,et al. The role of crosstalk in dual-task performance: evidence from manipulating response-code overlap , 2009, Psychological research.
[78] Axel Buchner,et al. Learning versus behavioral expression of the learned: The effects of a secondary tone-counting task on implicit learning in the serial reaction task , 1998 .
[79] David E. Kieras,et al. VIRTUALLY PERFECT TIME SHARING IN DUAL-TASK PERFORMANCE: , 2001 .
[80] Dorit Wenke,et al. A secondary tone-counting task suppresses expression of knowledge in the serial reaction task , 1999 .
[81] E. Hazeltine,et al. Incidental learning and task boundaries. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[82] D. Shanks. IMPLICIT LEARNING AND TACIT KNOWLEDGE - AN ESSAY ON THE COGNITIVE UNCONSCIOUS - REBER,A , 1995 .
[83] I. Koch,et al. Higher-Order Cognitive Control in Dual Tasks: Evidence From Task-Pair Switching , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[84] Z. Dienes,et al. Gambling on the unconscious: A comparison of wagering and confidence ratings as measures of awareness in an artificial grammar task , 2010, Consciousness and Cognition.
[85] A. Reber,et al. Another wrinkle on the dual-task SRT experiment: It’s probably not dual task , 2000, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[86] Ronald Hübner,et al. Strategic capacity sharing between two tasks: evidence from tasks with the same and with different task sets , 2009, Psychological research.
[87] Dieter Nattkemper,et al. The role of anticipation and intention in the learning of effects of self-performed actions , 2004, Psychological research.
[88] Tim Curran,et al. Attentional and Nonattentional Forms of Sequence Learning , 1993 .
[89] E. Hazeltine,et al. Investigating perfect timesharing: the relationship between IM-compatible tasks and dual-task performance. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[90] R. Gómez. Variability and Detection of Invariant Structure , 2002, Psychological science.
[91] Markus Janczyk,et al. Sequential modulation of backward crosstalk and task-shielding in dual-tasking. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[92] Fang Zhao,et al. Differential effects of cue-based and sequence knowledge-based predictability on multitasking performance. , 2018, Acta psychologica.
[93] A. Clark. Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. , 2013, The Behavioral and brain sciences.
[94] Rico Fischer,et al. Predicting high levels of multitasking reduces between-tasks interactions. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[95] M. Ziessler,et al. Learning of event sequences is based on response-effect learning: further evidence from a serial reaction task. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[96] C. Eriksen,et al. Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .
[97] E. Hazeltine,et al. Corresponding Authors: Hierarchical Task Representation: Task Files and Response Selection Task-file Representation Task Files Account for Interactions between Discrete Responses , 2022 .
[98] Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al. Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .
[99] C. W. Telford. The refractory phase of voluntary and associative responses , 1931 .
[100] Hillary D. Schwarb,et al. Parallel response selection disrupts sequence learning under dual-task conditions. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[101] Markus Janczyk,et al. Identifying the Locus of Compatibility-Based Backward Crosstalk: Evidence From an Extended PRP Paradigm , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[102] Matthew J. C. Crump,et al. In Support of a Distinction between Voluntary and Stimulus-Driven Control: A Review of the Literature on Proportion Congruent Effects , 2012, Front. Psychology.
[103] J. C. Johnston,et al. Vanishing dual-task interference after practice: has the bottleneck been eliminated or is it merely latent? , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[104] Markus Janczyk,et al. The Motor Locus of No-Go Backward Crosstalk , 2018, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[105] Herbert Heuer,et al. Task integration as a factor in secondary-task effects on sequence learning , 1997 .
[106] P. Perruchet,et al. Implicit learning and statistical learning: one phenomenon, two approaches , 2006, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[107] Andrea M Philipp,et al. The role of inhibition in task switching: A review , 2010, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[108] T. Schubert,et al. No Evidence for Task Automatization After Dual-Task Training in Younger and Older Adults , 2017, Psychology and aging.
[109] K. Lashley. The problem of serial order in behavior , 1951 .
[110] R. Rescorla. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .
[111] A. Greenwald,et al. On doing two things at once. II. Elimination of the psychological refractory period effect. , 1973, Journal of experimental psychology.
[112] Robert Gaschler,et al. Implicit learning of what comes when and where within a sequence: The time-course of acquiring serial position-item and item-item associations to represent serial order , 2012, Advances in cognitive psychology.
[113] Dietrich Manzey,et al. Flexibility of individual multitasking strategies in task-switching with preview: are preferences for serial versus overlapping task processing dependent on between-task conflict? , 2017, Psychological Research.