High-intensity interval exercise impairs neuroelectric indices of reinforcement-learning

A single bout of high-intensity interval exercise (HIIE) improves behavioural measures of cognitive function; however, investigations using event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine the systems that underlie these cognitive improvements are lacking. The reward positivity is a positive-going ERP component that indexes reward processing evoked by 'win' feedback and is a candidate marker of an underlying human reinforcement learning system. While HIIE improves behavioural measures of learning, it is unknown how HIIE affects the amplitude of the reward positivity. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate how HIIE affects reward positivity amplitude in response to reward feedback in university students. Using a single-group randomly assigned counterbalance crossover design, 25 healthy university students performed HIIE and control visits on separate days. Electroencephalographic data was recorded before (pre-intervention) and 10 min after (post-intervention) the intervention period while participants played a novel gambling task. The HIIE intervention consisted of 4 separate body-weight exercises totaling 11 min in duration, including rest. The control visit intervention consisted of quietly watching a nature documentary for 11 min. Heart rate (HR) was measured at the same time intervals in both trials. Analysis revealed that HIIE significantly diminished the amplitude of the reward positivity whereas it remained unaffected in the control condition. HR was significantly higher following HIIE compared to control during post-intervention testing. These findings suggest that mechanisms of reinforcement learning are impaired shortly after HIIE cessation, possibly due to persistent, suboptimal arousal as evidenced by elevated HR post-HIIE.

[1]  T. Hamaoka,et al.  Greater impact of acute high-intensity interval exercise on post-exercise executive function compared to moderate-intensity continuous exercise , 2016, Physiology & Behavior.

[2]  C. Ritz,et al.  Acute Exercise and Motor Memory Consolidation: The Role of Exercise Timing , 2016, Neural plasticity.

[3]  Hitoo Nishino,et al.  Striatal dopamine turnover during treadmill running in the rat: Relation to the speed of running , 1994, Brain Research Bulletin.

[4]  B. Gurd,et al.  Four minutes of in-class high-intensity interval activity improves selective attention in 9- to 11-year olds. , 2015, Applied physiology, nutrition, and metabolism = Physiologie appliquee, nutrition et metabolisme.

[5]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Hypersensitivity to Reward in Problem Gamblers , 2010, Biological Psychiatry.

[6]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task. , 2007, Psychophysiology.

[7]  D. Sutoo,et al.  Regulation of brain function by exercise , 2003, Neurobiology of Disease.

[8]  M. Delgado,et al.  Acute Stress Influences Neural Circuits of Reward Processing , 2012, Frontiers in neuroscience.

[9]  Peter Dayan,et al.  A Neural Substrate of Prediction and Reward , 1997, Science.

[10]  S. Knecht,et al.  High impact running improves learning , 2007, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[11]  P. Tomporowski,et al.  The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis , 2010, Brain Research.

[12]  J. Bhattacharya,et al.  High-Learners Present Larger Mid-Frontal Theta Power and Connectivity in Response to Incorrect Performance Feedback , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[13]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Single bouts of exercise selectively sustain attentional processes. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[14]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.

[15]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  Motivation of extended behaviors by anterior cingulate cortex , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  D. Weinberger,et al.  How to be smart , 1999 .

[17]  M. Brand,et al.  Decision making under stress: A selective review , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[18]  Marc Roig,et al.  Acute exercise improves motor memory: Exploring potential biomarkers , 2014, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[19]  Sakshi,et al.  Effect of acute moderate exercise on cognitive P300 in persons having sedentary lifestyles , 2012, International journal of applied & basic medical research.

[20]  E. Kimes,et al.  Evaluation of Vancomycin TDM Strategies: Prediction and Prevention of Kidney Injuries Based on Vancomycin TDM Results , 2023, Journal of Korean medical science.

[21]  Jeremy J. Walsh,et al.  Brief, High-Intensity Interval Exercise Improves Selective Attention in University Students , 2018 .

[22]  C. Voelcker-Rehage,et al.  Intermittent maximal exercise improves attentional performance only in physically active students. , 2012, Archives of medical research.

[23]  Caroline Di Bernardi Luft,et al.  Learning from feedback: The neural mechanisms of feedback processing facilitating better performance , 2014, Behavioural Brain Research.

[24]  Ippeita Dan,et al.  Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: An fNIRS study , 2014, NeuroImage.

[25]  J. Lygeros,et al.  Decision Making I , 2014 .

[26]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The feedback correct-related positivity: sensitivity of the event-related brain potential to unexpected positive feedback. , 2008, Psychophysiology.

[27]  R. Cabeza,et al.  Imaging Cognition: An Empirical Review of PET Studies with Normal Subjects , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[28]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  The neural basis of error detection: conflict monitoring and the error-related negativity. , 2004, Psychological review.

[29]  Chad C. Williams,et al.  Choosing MUSE: Validation of a Low-Cost, Portable EEG System for ERP Research , 2017, Front. Neurosci..

[30]  C. Hillman,et al.  Comparison of the acute effects of high-intensity interval training and continuous aerobic walking on inhibitory control. , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[31]  C. Hillman,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging in Exercise and Sport Sciences , 2012, Springer New York.

[32]  C. Braun,et al.  Event-Related Brain Potentials Following Incorrect Feedback in a Time-Estimation Task: Evidence for a Generic Neural System for Error Detection , 1997, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  T. McMorris,et al.  Beyond the Catecholamines Hypothesis for an Acute Exercise–Cognition Interaction: A Neurochemical Perspective , 2016 .

[34]  S. Matsumura,et al.  Increased noradrenergic activity in the ventromedial hypothalamus during treadmill running in rats. , 2010, Journal of nutritional science and vitaminology.

[35]  Yue-Jia Luo,et al.  Anxiety and feedback negativity. , 2010, Psychophysiology.

[36]  Matthew B. Pontifex,et al.  Acute Aerobic Exercise Effects on Event-related Brain Potentials , 2009 .

[37]  Hagar Kandel,et al.  Arousal , 2020, Encyclopedia of Behavioral Medicine.

[38]  H. Critchley,et al.  Cerebral correlates of autonomic cardiovascular arousal: a functional neuroimaging investigation in humans , 2000, The Journal of physiology.

[39]  Clay B. Holroyd,et al.  The neural basis of human error processing: reinforcement learning, dopamine, and the error-related negativity. , 2002, Psychological review.

[40]  J. Polich,et al.  P300, cognitive capability, and personality: A correlational study of university undergraduates , 1992 .

[41]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Show me the Money: The impact of actual rewards and losses on the feedback negativity , 2014, Brain and Cognition.

[42]  A Pfefferbaum,et al.  P300 and long-term memory: latency predicts recognition performance. , 1985, Psychophysiology.

[43]  Olave E Krigolson,et al.  Event-related brain potentials and the study of reward processing: Methodological considerations. , 2017, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[44]  A. Diamond Executive functions. , 2014, Handbook of clinical neurology.

[45]  H. C. Webster To The Better , 2019, On a Darkling Plain.

[46]  Thomas F Münte,et al.  Internal and external information in error processing , 2008, BMC Neuroscience.

[47]  Adrian H. Taylor,et al.  Acute exercise modulates cigarette cravings and brain activation in response to smoking-related images: an fMRI study , 2009, Psychopharmacology.

[48]  Shawn M Arent,et al.  Arousal, Anxiety, and Performance: A Reexamination of the Inverted-U Hypothesis , 2003, Research quarterly for exercise and sport.

[49]  John N. J. Reynolds,et al.  Dopamine-dependent plasticity of corticostriatal synapses , 2002, Neural Networks.

[50]  Caterina Pesce,et al.  Antecedent acute cycling exercise affects attention control: an ERP study using attention network test , 2015, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[51]  J. C. Basso,et al.  The Effects of Acute Exercise on Mood, Cognition, Neurophysiology, and Neurochemical Pathways: A Review , 2017, Brain plasticity.

[52]  J. Markworth,et al.  Metabolic and hormonal responses to isoenergetic high-intensity interval exercise and continuous moderate-intensity exercise. , 2014, American journal of physiology. Endocrinology and metabolism.

[53]  Karl T. Schmidt,et al.  Aerobic exercise decreases the positive-reinforcing effects of cocaine. , 2008, Drug and alcohol dependence.

[54]  James W. Tanaka,et al.  Learning to Become an Expert: Reinforcement Learning and the Acquisition of Perceptual Expertise , 2009, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[55]  Miss A.O. Penney (b) , 1974, The New Yale Book of Quotations.

[56]  W. Staines,et al.  Acute exercise enhances the response to paired associative stimulation-induced plasticity in the primary motor cortex , 2014, Experimental Brain Research.

[57]  Arne Dietrich,et al.  The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[58]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Decision making, the P3, and the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system. , 2005, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  T. McMorris,et al.  A test of the catecholamines hypothesis for an acute exercise–cognition interaction , 2008, Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior.

[60]  P Salmon,et al.  Effects of physical exercise on anxiety, depression, and sensitivity to stress: a unifying theory. , 2001, Clinical psychology review.

[61]  Tea Lulic,et al.  Physical activity levels determine exercise-induced changes in brain excitability , 2017, PloS one.

[62]  Gao-Xia Wei,et al.  Acute Aerobic Exercise Increases Cortical Activity during Working Memory: A Functional MRI Study in Female College Students , 2014, PloS one.

[63]  Greg H. Proudfit The reward positivity: from basic research on reward to a biomarker for depression. , 2015, Psychophysiology.

[64]  Rita Z. Goldstein,et al.  Impaired Neural Response to Negative Prediction Errors in Cocaine Addiction , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[65]  B. Alderman,et al.  Acute exercise has a general facilitative effect on cognitive function: A combined ERP temporal dynamics and BDNF study. , 2017, Psychophysiology.

[66]  A. Kramer,et al.  Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition , 2008, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[67]  Dan Foti,et al.  Reward dysfunction in major depression: Multimodal neuroimaging evidence for refining the melancholic phenotype , 2014, NeuroImage.

[68]  Masako Okamoto,et al.  Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test , 2010, NeuroImage.

[69]  J. Etnier,et al.  The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis , 2012, Brain Research.

[70]  Anna Weinberg,et al.  Event‐related potential activity in the basal ganglia differentiates rewards from nonrewards: Response to commentary , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[71]  W. Sommer,et al.  The better, the bigger: The effect of graded positive performance feedback on the reward positivity , 2016, Biological Psychology.

[72]  C. Ritz,et al.  Acute Exercise and Motor Memory Consolidation: The Role of Exercise Intensity , 2016, PloS one.

[73]  Mark Rakobowchuk,et al.  Similar metabolic adaptations during exercise after low volume sprint interval and traditional endurance training in humans , 2008, The Journal of physiology.

[74]  Tetsuo Kida,et al.  Differential influences of exercise intensity on information processing in the central nervous system , 2004, European Journal of Applied Physiology.

[75]  S. Rauch,et al.  The counting stroop: An interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging—validation study with functional MRI , 1998, Human brain mapping.

[76]  H. Roschel,et al.  Influence of Acute High-Intensity Aerobic Interval Exercise Bout on Selective Attention and Short-Term Memory Tasks , 2014, Perceptual and motor skills.

[77]  C. Hillman,et al.  Cardiorespiratory fitness and acute aerobic exercise effects on neuroelectric and behavioral measures of action monitoring , 2006, Neuroscience.

[78]  Y. Nishihira,et al.  The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.

[79]  M. Fleshner,et al.  Long-term voluntary wheel running is rewarding and produces plasticity in the mesolimbic reward pathway , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[80]  M. Kouzaki,et al.  Effects of moderate-intensity endurance and high-intensity intermittent training on anaerobic capacity and VO2max. , 1996, Medicine and science in sports and exercise.

[81]  C. J. Cooper Anatomical and physiological mechanisms of arousal, with special reference to the effects of exercise. , 1973, Ergonomics.

[82]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Learning from experience: Event-related potential correlates of reward processing, neural adaptation, and behavioral choice , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[83]  I. Daum,et al.  Learning‐related changes in reward expectancy are reflected in the feedback‐related negativity , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[84]  Marc Roig,et al.  A Single Bout of Exercise Improves Motor Memory , 2012, PloS one.