Temporal synchrony is an effective cue for grouping and segmentation in the absence of form cues.
暂无分享,去创建一个
Mark Edwards | Alan Johnston | Reuben Rideaux | David R Badcock | A. Johnston | D. Badcock | Reuben Rideaux | Mark Edwards
[1] D H HUBEL,et al. RECEPTIVE FIELDS AND FUNCTIONAL ARCHITECTURE IN TWO NONSTRIATE VISUAL AREAS (18 AND 19) OF THE CAT. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.
[2] Patrick T. Goodbourn,et al. Spatial limitations of fast temporal segmentation are best modeled by V1 receptive fields. , 2013, Journal of vision.
[3] S. Dakin,et al. Snakes and ladders: the role of temporal modulation in visual contour integration , 2001, Vision Research.
[4] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Filtering Reveals Form in Temporally Structured Displays , 1999 .
[5] J. Movshon,et al. Cortical oscillatory responses do not affect visual segmentation , 1996, Vision Research.
[6] Mark Edwards,et al. The detection of multiple global directions: capacity limits with spatially segregated and transparent-motion signals. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[7] P. Milner. A model for visual shape recognition. , 1974, Psychological review.
[8] N. Prins. The psi-marginal adaptive method: How to give nuisance parameters the attention they deserve (no more, no less). , 2013, Journal of vision.
[9] D. Badcock,et al. Discrimination of global-motion signal strength , 1998, Vision Research.
[10] J Edwin Dickinson,et al. Selective attention contributes to global processing in vision. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[11] K. H. Britten,et al. Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.
[12] Farid I. Kandil,et al. Figure–ground segregation can rely on differences in motion direction , 2004, Vision Research.
[13] E. Vaadia,et al. Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the frontal cortex of behaving monkeys. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.
[14] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.
[15] Randolph Blake,et al. Neural synergy in visual grouping: when good continuation meets common fate , 2001, Vision Research.
[16] R Blake,et al. Visual form created solely from temporal structure. , 1999, Science.
[17] Christoph von der Malsburg,et al. The Correlation Theory of Brain Function , 1994 .
[18] Mark Edwards,et al. Role of form information in motion pooling and segmentation. , 2015, Journal of vision.
[19] S. Nishida,et al. Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.
[20] Marius Usher,et al. Visual synchrony affects binding and segmentation in perception , 1998, Nature.
[21] W. Newsome,et al. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[22] R. Blake,et al. Visual features that vary together over time group together over space , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.
[23] UTE LEONARDS,et al. The Influence of Temporal Phase Differences on Texture Segmentation , 1996, Vision Research.
[24] D H Brainard,et al. The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[25] E. Castet,et al. High temporal frequency synchrony is insufficient for perceptual grouping , 2002, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[26] Virginie van Wassenhove,et al. Temporal Structure in Audiovisual Sensory Selection , 2012, PloS one.
[27] C. Koch,et al. Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness , 1990 .
[28] Edward H. Adelson,et al. Synchrony does not promote grouping in temporally structured displays , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[29] George Sperling,et al. The information available in brief visual presentations. , 1960 .
[30] Randolph Blake,et al. The role of temporal structure in human vision. , 2005, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.
[31] R. Efron,et al. The visual persistence of a moving stroboscopically illuminated object. , 1971, The American journal of psychology.
[32] C. Tyler,et al. Bayesian adaptive estimation of psychometric slope and threshold , 1999, Vision Research.
[33] D G Pelli,et al. The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.
[34] Isamu Motoyoshi,et al. The role of spatial interactions in perceptual synchrony. , 2004, Journal of vision.
[35] Jon Driver,et al. Edge-Assignment and Figure–Ground Segmentation in Short-Term Visual Matching , 1996, Cognitive Psychology.
[36] R Efron,et al. The minimum duration of a perception. , 1970, Neuropsychologia.
[37] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[38] Mark Edwards,et al. Common-fate motion processing: Interaction of the On and Off pathways , 2009, Vision Research.
[39] Jean Lorenceau,et al. Superposition catastrophe and form-motion binding. , 2008, Journal of vision.
[40] Ad Aertsen,et al. Stable propagation of synchronous spiking in cortical neural networks , 1999, Nature.
[41] E. Averbach,et al. Short-term memory in vision , 1961 .
[42] J. Ross,et al. Spatial limitations of temporal segmentation , 1999, Vision Research.
[43] Hany Farid,et al. Temporal synchrony in perceptual grouping: a critique , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[44] K Nakayama,et al. Stereoscopic Depth: Its Relation to Image Segmentation, Grouping, and the Recognition of Occluded Objects , 1989, Perception.
[45] I Kovács,et al. A closed curve is much more than an incomplete one: effect of closure in figure-ground segmentation. , 1993, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.