Neural regions discriminating contextual information as conveyed through the learned preferences of others

The human brain consists of a network of regions that are engaged when one observes the movements of others. Observing unexpected movements, as defined by the context, often elicits greater activity, particularly in the right posterior superior temporal sulcus (pSTS). This implies that observers use contextual information to form expectations about an agent’s goal and subsequent movements. The current study sought to identify regions that support the formation of these context-dependent expectations, with the pSTS being one candidate, given the consistent contextual modulation of its activity. We presented participants with fictitious individuals who had emotion-dependent food preferences, and instructed participants to indicate which food they expected each individual to choose based on the individual’s current emotional state. Each individual’s preference and emotional state therefore created a context that informed the observer’s expectation of the individual’s choice. Multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA) was used to assess if these different contexts could be discriminated in the pSTS and elsewhere in the brain. No evidence for context discrimination was found in the pSTS. Context discrimination was found instead a network of other brain regions including the anterior medial prefrontal cortex (amPFC), bilateral parietal cortex, left middle temporal gyrus (L MTG) and left anterior temporal lobe (L ATL), which have been previously associated with context processing, and semantic and memory retrieval. All together, these regions possibly support the formation of context-dependent expectations of an agent’s goal.

[1]  Johan D. Carlin,et al.  Choosing the Rules: Distinct and Overlapping Frontoparietal Representations of Task Rules for Perceptual Decisions , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  The neurobiology of semantic memory , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[3]  Elizabeth Jefferies,et al.  Semantic Processing in the Anterior Temporal Lobes: A Meta-analysis of the Functional Neuroimaging Literature , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[4]  M. Brass,et al.  Investigating Action Understanding: Inferential Processes versus Action Simulation , 2007, Current Biology.

[5]  D. R. Euston,et al.  The Role of Medial Prefrontal Cortex in Memory and Decision Making , 2012, Neuron.

[6]  Stefan Pollmann,et al.  PyMVPA: a Python Toolbox for Multivariate Pattern Analysis of fMRI Data , 2009, Neuroinformatics.

[7]  Doris Y. Tsao,et al.  Single-Unit Recordings in the Macaque Face Patch System Reveal Limitations of fMRI MVPA , 2015, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[8]  John Duncan,et al.  Multi-voxel coding of stimuli, rules, and responses in human frontoparietal cortex , 2011, NeuroImage.

[9]  J. Weber,et al.  Journal Section: Behavioral/systems/cognitive Social Cognitive Conflict Resolution: Contributions of Domain General and Domain Specific Neural Systems , 2022 .

[10]  G. McCarthy,et al.  Functional Heterogeneity and Convergence in the Right Temporoparietal Junction. , 2016, Cerebral cortex.

[11]  Demis Hassabis,et al.  Imagine all the people: how the brain creates and uses personality models to predict behavior. , 2014, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Action representation in the superior temporal sulcus in children and adults: An fMRI study , 2012, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[13]  B Lyman,et al.  The nutritional values and food group characteristics of foods preferred during various emotions. , 1982, The Journal of psychology.

[14]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  Distributed representations of dynamic facial expressions in the superior temporal sulcus. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[15]  D. Perrett,et al.  A region of right posterior superior temporal sulcus responds to observed intentional actions , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[16]  Aina Puce,et al.  Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[17]  T. Allison,et al.  Brain activation evoked by perception of gaze shifts: the influence of context , 2003, Neuropsychologia.

[18]  Andrew D. Engell,et al.  Probabilistic atlases for face and biological motion perception: An analysis of their reliability and overlap , 2013, NeuroImage.

[19]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The proactive brain: memory for predictions , 2009, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[20]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  Deconvolving BOLD activation in event-related designs for multivoxel pattern classification analyses , 2012, NeuroImage.

[21]  Brice A. Kuhl,et al.  Successful Remembering Elicits Event-Specific Activity Patterns in Lateral Parietal Cortex , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[22]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  Kevin A. Pelphrey,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others: The Perceived Intentionality of an Action Influences Activity in the Superior Temporal Sulcus during Social Perception , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[24]  Suk Won Han,et al.  Functional Fractionation of the Stimulus-Driven Attention Network , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Episodic retrieval and the cortical binding of relational activity , 2011, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[26]  David M. Sobel,et al.  Action Understanding in the Superior Temporal Sulcus Region , 2009, Psychological science.

[27]  Laura Monetta,et al.  The role of the left anterior temporal lobe in the semantic processing of famous faces , 2010, NeuroImage.

[28]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[29]  Raja Parasuraman,et al.  Attention, biological motion, and action recognition , 2012, NeuroImage.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Predictive coding: an account of the mirror neuron system , 2007, Cognitive Processing.

[31]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Goal Representation in Human Anterior Intraparietal Sulcus , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  M. Corbetta,et al.  The Reorienting System of the Human Brain: From Environment to Theory of Mind , 2008, Neuron.

[33]  A M Dale,et al.  Optimal experimental design for event‐related fMRI , 1999, Human brain mapping.

[34]  R. Saxe,et al.  Theory of Mind: A Neural Prediction Problem , 2013, Neuron.

[35]  J. Tanaka,et al.  The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants , 2009, Psychiatry Research.

[36]  Yi Chen,et al.  Statistical inference and multiple testing correction in classification-based multi-voxel pattern analysis (MVPA): Random permutations and cluster size control , 2011, NeuroImage.

[37]  C. Grady,et al.  Event-related fMRI studies of episodic encoding and retrieval: Meta-analyses using activation likelihood estimation , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  G. McCarthy,et al.  The posterior superior temporal sulcus is sensitive to the outcome of human and non-human goal-directed actions. , 2011, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[39]  William W. Graves,et al.  Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[40]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.