La perception visuelle des orientations et« l'effet de l'oblique »

This paper surveys studies on the anisotropic perception of orientation in the visual system. The vertical and horizontal orientations are known to be perceived more accurately than oblique orientations. This anisotropy, which called the oblique effect, was present in a large variety of tasks (detection, discrimination, recognition, memorisation) . The discussion about the visual oblique effect (VOE) concerns the nature and level of the processes that generate this effect. It is still unknown whether the VOE stems from low levels of the visual processing system or/and from higher levels. The « low level hypothesis » explains the VOE by the neural properties existing at low levels of visual processing. This hypothesis is supported by three types of arguments : 1 I the VOE is explained by the axis-dependent change in the properties of orientation- selective neurons early in the visual hierarchy (the neurons that are tuned to horizontal and vertical orientations might be more sensitive, narrowly tuned or/and more numerous than those tuned to oblique orientations) ; 2 I the VOE is not influenced by factors independent of the visual stimulus (e.g. : environment, etc.) ; 3 I the VOE follows a retinal reference frame when the head or body of subjects is tilted with respect to gravity. The hypothesis of a « high level VOE » is supported by opposite arguments : 1 I the observations fail to follow the predictions of neuronal models ; 2 I the magnitude of the VOE is influenced by some factors independent of the visual stimulus ; 3 I the VOE follows a gravitational rather than a retinal reference frame in some experiments. The analyse of contradictory results shows that the origin of the visual oblique effects seems to be multi-component and to occur at different levels of processing according to the nature of task.

[1]  Y. Hatwell,et al.  Toucher pour connaître , 2000 .

[2]  C. Thinus-Blanc,et al.  Representation of space in blind persons: vision as a spatial sense? , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[3]  Keiji Tanaka,et al.  Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. , 1996, Annual review of neuroscience.

[4]  Edouard Gentaz,et al.  The Reproduction of Vertical and Oblique Orientations in the Visual, Haptic, and Somatovestibular Systems , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[5]  R Vogels,et al.  Decision factors affecting line orientation judgments in the method of single stimuli , 1986, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  E. DeYoe,et al.  Concurrent processing in the primate visual cortex. , 1995 .

[7]  R. L. Valois,et al.  The orientation and direction selectivity of cells in macaque visual cortex , 1982, Vision Research.

[8]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.

[9]  Marc Green,et al.  The oblique effect in a vernier acuity situation , 1977 .

[10]  G. Orban,et al.  Human perceptual learning in identifying the oblique orientation: retinotopy, orientation specificity and monocularity. , 1995, The Journal of physiology.

[11]  S. Lehmkuhle,et al.  The Oblique Effects of Pattern and Flicker Sensitivity: Implications for Mixed Physiological Input , 1982, Perception.

[12]  H. Ross,et al.  Genetic and Environmental Factors in Orientation Anisotropy: A Field Study in the British Isles , 1979, Perception.

[13]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Orientation anisotropy in vernier acuity , 1995, Vision Research.

[14]  B N Timney,et al.  Orientation anisotropy: incidence and magnitude in Caucasian and Chinese subjects. , 1976, Science.

[15]  H. Pashler,et al.  Improvement in line orientation discrimination is retinally local but dependent on cognitive set , 1992, Perception & psychophysics.

[16]  J. S. Wright,et al.  The oblique effect in orientation acuity , 1997, Vision Research.

[17]  É. Gentaz Existe-t-il un « effet de l'oblique » dans la perception tactile des orientations? , 2000 .

[18]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Identification of symmetry: Effects of stimulus orientation and head position , 1982, Perception & psychophysics.

[19]  G. Orban,et al.  The influence of eccentricity on receptive field types and orientation selectivity in areas 17 and 18 of the cat , 1981, Brain Research.

[20]  R. Mansfield,et al.  Neural Basis of Orientation Perception in Primate Vision , 1974, Science.

[21]  C. Fisher Children's memory for orientation in the absence of external cues. , 1979, Child development.

[22]  J P Thomas,et al.  Bandwidths of orientation channels in human vision. , 1979, Journal of the Optical Society of America.

[23]  D. Ts'o,et al.  Visual topography in primate V2: multiple representation across functional stripes , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  G. Orban,et al.  The effect of practice on the oblique effect in line orientation judgments , 1985, Vision Research.

[25]  M. Tanaka,et al.  Visual properties and spatial distribution of neurones in the visual association area on the prelunate gyrus of the awake monkey , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  The visual field representation in striate cortex of the macaque monkey: Asymmetries, anisotropies, and individual variability , 1984, Vision Research.

[27]  M. Morgan On the scaling of size judgements by orientational cues , 1992, Vision Research.

[28]  D. Buonomano,et al.  Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[29]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Recognition of stimulus orientation , 1990, Vision Research.

[30]  R. Andersen,et al.  The thalamic relations of the caudal inferior parietal lobule and the lateral prefrontal cortex in monkeys: Divergent cortical projections from cell clusters in the medial pulvinar nucleus , 1985, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[31]  M. Banks,et al.  The effect of head tilt on meridional differences in acuity: Implications for orientation constancy , 1975 .

[32]  E. R. Siqueland,et al.  Discrimination of Orientation by Human Infants , 1981, Perception.

[33]  D. Levi,et al.  Meridional Anisotropy in the Discrimination of Parallel and Perpendicular Lines—Effect of Body Tilt , 1996, Perception.

[34]  P. Bryant,et al.  Perception and Memory of the Orientation of Visually Presented Lines by Children , 1969, Nature.

[35]  Orientation anisotropy: some caveats in interpreting group differences and developmental changes , 1992, Ophthalmic & physiological optics : the journal of the British College of Ophthalmic Opticians.

[36]  B. C. Motter,et al.  Common and differential effects of attentive fixation on the excitability of parietal and prestriate (V4) cortical visual neurons in the macaque monkey , 1987, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[37]  C. Blakemore,et al.  An analysis of orientation selectivity in the cat's visual cortex , 1974, Experimental Brain Research.

[38]  G. Orban,et al.  Human orientation discrimination tested with long stimuli , 1984, Vision Research.

[39]  C. Blakemore,et al.  Does the vestibular apparatus play a role in the development of the visual system? , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[40]  R. Held,et al.  Visual acuity and its meridional variations in children aged 7–60 months , 1983, Vision Research.

[41]  S. Thorpe,et al.  Dynamics of orientation coding in area V1 of the awake primate , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[42]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[43]  F. Attneave,et al.  Voluntary control of frame of reference and slope equivalence under head rotation. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[44]  C. Gross,et al.  Visuotopic organization and extent of V3 and V4 of the macaque , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[45]  Dennis M. Levi,et al.  Visibility, luminance and vernier acuity , 1993, Vision Research.

[46]  E. Essock Anisotropies of perceived contrast and detection speed , 1982, Vision Research.

[47]  C. Fisher Children's memory for line orientation: a reexamination of the "oblique effect". , 1980, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[48]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Shifter circuits: a computational strategy for dynamic aspects of visual processing. , 1987, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[49]  I. Rock The Logic of Perception , 1983 .

[50]  F. Attneave,et al.  Discriminability of stimuli varying in physical and retinal orientation , 1967 .

[51]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Orientation acuity estimated with simultaneous and successive procedures. , 1992, Spatial vision.

[52]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Pathways for motion analysis: Cortical connections of the medial superior temporal and fundus of the superior temporal visual areas in the macaque , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[53]  Ian P. Howard,et al.  Human visual orientation , 1982 .

[54]  K. Rockland,et al.  Direct temporal-occipital feedback connections to striate cortex (V1) in the macaque monkey. , 1994, Cerebral cortex.

[55]  Edward A. Essock,et al.  The influence of stimulus length on the oblique effect of contrast sensitivity , 1990, Vision Research.

[56]  Jeffrey A. Sloan,et al.  Spatial frequency analysis of the visual environment: Anisotropy and the carpentered environment hypothesis , 1978, Vision Research.

[57]  C. Gross,et al.  Visual topography of V2 in the macaque , 1981, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[58]  S. Appelle Perception and discrimination as a function of stimulus orientation: the "oblique effect" in man and animals. , 1972, Psychological bulletin.

[59]  Children's memory for oblique orientation: a matter of degree? , 1982, Child development.

[60]  R Over,et al.  Detection and recognition of mirror-image obliques by young children. , 1967, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[61]  F. Campbell,et al.  The effect of orientation on the visual resolution of gratings , 1966, The Journal of physiology.

[62]  H H Emsley,et al.  Irregular astigmatism of the eye: effect of correcting lenses , 1925 .

[63]  Richard Held,et al.  Infant visual acuity and its meridional variation , 1978, Vision Research.

[64]  E. Essock The Oblique Effect of Stimulus Identification Considered with Respect to Two Classes of Oblique Effects , 1980, Perception.

[65]  D. Whitteridge,et al.  The representation of the visual field on the cerebral cortex in monkeys , 1961, The Journal of physiology.

[66]  R. Desimone,et al.  Visual areas in the temporal cortex of the macaque , 1979, Brain Research.

[67]  P. Bryant Discrimination of mirror images by young children. , 1973, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[68]  R. M. Siegel,et al.  Corticocortical connections of anatomically and physiologically defined subdivisions within the inferior parietal lobule , 1990, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[69]  Christian Marendaz,et al.  Visual Search for Oriented Lines: Anisotropy and Psychophysical Modeling Nature and Dynamics of Reference Frames Nature and Dynamics of Reference Frames in Visual Search for Orientation: Implications for Early Visual Processing , 2022 .

[70]  D W Heeley,et al.  Anisotropic Axes in Orientation Perception are Not Retinotopically Mapped , 1993, Perception.

[71]  D. W. Heeley,et al.  Orientation acuity for sine-wave gratings with random variation of spatial frequency , 1993, Vision Research.

[72]  The Role of Stimulus Alignment in Children's Memory for Line Orientation. , 1982 .

[73]  D. J. Felleman,et al.  Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. , 1991, Cerebral cortex.