Effects of reward on oculomotor control.

The present study examines the extent to which distractors that signal the availability of monetary reward on a given trial affect eye movements. We used a novel eye movement task in which observers had to follow a target around the screen while ignoring distractors presented at varying locations. We examined the effects of reward magnitude and distractor location on a host of oculomotor properties, including saccade latency, amplitude, landing position, curvature, and erroneous saccades toward the distractor. We found consistent effects of reward magnitude on classic oculomotor phenomena such as the remote distractor effect, the global effect, and oculomotor capture by the distractor. We also show that a distractor in the visual hemifield opposite to the target had a larger effect on oculomotor control than an equidistant distractor in the same hemifield as the target. Bayesian hierarchical drift diffusion modeling revealed large differences in drift rate depending on the reward value, location, and visual hemifield of the distractor stimulus. Our findings suggest that high reward distractors not only capture the eyes but also affect a multitude of oculomotor properties associated with oculomotor inhibition and control.

[1]  R. Desimone,et al.  Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4 , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  The effect of reward on orienting and reorienting in exogenous cuing , 2014, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[3]  L. Pessoa,et al.  Embedding Reward Signals into Perception and Cognition , 2010, Front. Neurosci..

[4]  J. Schall,et al.  Neural Control of Voluntary Movement Initiation , 1996, Science.

[5]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Robust Representation of Stable Object Values in the Oculomotor Basal Ganglia , 2012, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[6]  W. Brown Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies , 1912, Nature.

[7]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Programming of endogenous and exogenous saccades: evidence for a competitive integration model. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[8]  H. Deubel,et al.  Effect of remote distractors on saccade programming: evidence for an extended fixation zone. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[9]  Lauri Nummenmaa,et al.  Gaze distractors influence saccadic curvature: Evidence for the role of the oculomotor system in gaze-cued orienting , 2006, Vision Research.

[10]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  A competitive integration model of exogenous and endogenous eye movements , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.

[11]  N. Daw,et al.  Reinforcement Learning Signals in the Human Striatum Distinguish Learners from Nonlearners during Reward-Based Decision Making , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[12]  W. Newsome,et al.  Matching Behavior and the Representation of Value in the Parietal Cortex , 2004, Science.

[13]  R. Desimone,et al.  Responses of Neurons in Inferior Temporal Cortex during Memory- Guided Visual Search , 1998 .

[14]  Deborah J. Serrien,et al.  Decisions of Voluntary Action: What vs When , 2010, Frontiers in neuroscience.

[15]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Extrafoveal preview benefit during free-viewing visual search in the monkey. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[16]  Kae Nakamura,et al.  Basal ganglia orient eyes to reward. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[17]  R. Klein,et al.  A Model of Saccade Initiation Based on the Competitive Integration of Exogenous and Endogenous Signals in the Superior Colliculus , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[18]  J. Findlay Global visual processing for saccadic eye movements , 1982, Vision Research.

[19]  Jacqueline Gottlieb,et al.  Attention, Reward, and Information Seeking , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[20]  M. Goldberg,et al.  Attention, intention, and priority in the parietal lobe. , 2010, Annual review of neuroscience.

[21]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of the basal ganglia in the control of purposive saccadic eye movements. , 2000, Physiological reviews.

[22]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Exogenous visual orienting by reward. , 2014, Journal of vision.

[23]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Context and competition in the capture of visual attention , 2011, Attention, perception & psychophysics.

[24]  David M Milstein,et al.  The Influence of Expected Value on Saccadic Preparation , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[25]  G. B. Shattuck THE EXPEDITION TO THE BAHAMA ISLANDS OF THE GEOGRAPHICAL SOCIETY OF BALTIMORE. , 1903, Science.

[26]  Okihide Hikosaka,et al.  Role of Primate Substantia Nigra Pars Reticulata in Reward-Oriented Saccadic Eye Movement , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[27]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Role of Tonically Active Neurons in Primate Caudate in Reward-Oriented Saccadic Eye Movement , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[28]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Visual and oculomotor functions of monkey substantia nigra pars reticulata. IV. Relation of substantia nigra to superior colliculus. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[29]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Early and late modulation of saccade deviations by target distractor similarity. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Patrick Cavanagh,et al.  Within-Hemifield Competition in Early Visual Areas Limits the Ability to Track Multiple Objects with Attention , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  The influence of attending to multiple locations on eye movements , 2005, Vision Research.

[32]  R Iansek,et al.  Motor functions of the basal ganglia , 1993, Psychological research.

[33]  J. A. Gisbergen,et al.  An analysis of curvature in fast and slow human saccades , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Modulation of saccadic eye movements by predicted reward outcome , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[35]  Steven Yantis,et al.  Learned Value Magnifies Salience-Based Attentional Capture , 2011, PloS one.

[36]  A. Lévy-schoen Détermination et latence de la réponse oculomotrice à deux stimulus simultanés ou successifs selon leur excentricité relative , 1969 .

[37]  L. Chelazzi,et al.  Behavioral/systems/cognitive Reward Changes Salience in Human Vision via the Anterior Cingulate , 2022 .

[38]  R. Walker,et al.  Curved saccade trajectories: Voluntary and reflexive saccades curve away from irrelevant distractors , 2001, Experimental Brain Research.

[39]  Patryk A. Laurent,et al.  Value-driven attentional capture , 2011, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[40]  W. Zoest,et al.  Reward-associated stimuli capture the eyes in spite of strategic attentional set , 2013, Vision Research.

[41]  D. Munoz,et al.  Competitive Integration of Visual and Preparatory Signals in the Superior Colliculus during Saccadic Programming , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[42]  M. Goldberg,et al.  The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex , 1998, Nature.

[43]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Reward grabs the eye: Oculomotor capture by rewarding stimuli , 2012, Vision Research.

[44]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: a failed theoretical dichotomy , 2012, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[45]  D. Rubin,et al.  Inference from Iterative Simulation Using Multiple Sequences , 1992 .

[46]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Independent Resources for Attentional Tracking in the Left and Right Visual Hemifields , 2005, Psychological science.

[47]  D. E. Irwin,et al.  Our Eyes do Not Always Go Where we Want Them to Go: Capture of the Eyes by New Objects , 1998 .

[48]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  A visual salience map in the primate frontal eye field. , 2005, Progress in brain research.

[49]  Steven A. Hillyard,et al.  Independent hemispheric attentional systems mediate visual search in split-brain patients , 1989, Nature.

[50]  Shinya Yamamoto,et al.  Reward Value-Contingent Changes of Visual Responses in the Primate Caudate Tail Associated with a Visuomotor Skill , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Reward alters the perception of time , 2016, Cognition.

[52]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  Modeling Response Times for Two-Choice Decisions , 1998 .

[53]  L M Chalupa,et al.  Visual receptive fields in the striate-recipient zone of the lateral posterior-pulvinar complex , 1989, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  S. Coren,et al.  Effect of Non-Target Stimuli upon Length of Voluntary Saccades , 1972, Perceptual and motor skills.

[55]  D. Munoz,et al.  Look away: the anti-saccade task and the voluntary control of eye movement , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[56]  E. Thorndike Animal Intelligence; Experimental Studies , 2009 .

[57]  Thomas V. Wiecki,et al.  HDDM: Hierarchical Bayesian estimation of the Drift-Diffusion Model in Python , 2013, Front. Neuroinform..

[58]  Richard P. Heitz,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Speed-Accuracy Tradeoff , 2012, Neuron.

[59]  O. Hikosaka,et al.  Expectation of reward modulates cognitive signals in the basal ganglia , 1998, Nature Neuroscience.

[60]  Jeffrey N. Rouder,et al.  A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[61]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  OpenSesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences , 2011, Behavior Research Methods.

[62]  Denis Cousineau,et al.  Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method , 2005 .

[63]  David E. Irwin,et al.  Influence of attentional capture on oculomotor control. , 1999, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[64]  J. Theeuwes,et al.  Distractors that signal reward attract the eyes , 2015 .

[65]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[66]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Oculomotor capture by stimuli that signal the availability of reward. , 2015, Journal of neurophysiology.

[67]  M. Masson,et al.  Using confidence intervals in within-subject designs , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[68]  Tom Beesley,et al.  When goals conflict with values: counterproductive attentional and oculomotor capture by reward-related stimuli. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[69]  Ana Torralbo,et al.  Perceptual-Load-Induced Selection as a Result of Local Competitive Interactions in Visual Cortex , 2008, Psychological science.

[70]  Jan Theeuwes,et al.  Sensitivity to value-driven attention is predicted by how we learn from value , 2017, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[71]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  Connectionist and diffusion models of reaction time. , 1999, Psychological review.

[72]  L. Stark,et al.  The main sequence, a tool for studying human eye movements , 1975 .