Early Social Experience Affects Neural Activity to Affiliative Facial Gestures in Newborn Nonhuman Primates

A fundamental issue in cognitive neuroscience is how the brain encodes the actions and intentions of others. The discovery of an action-production-perception mechanism underpinning such a capacity advanced our knowledge of how these processes occur; however, no study has examined how the early postnatal environment may shape action-production-perception. Here, we examined the effects of social experience on action-production-perception in 3-day-old rhesus macaques that were raised either with or without their biological mothers. We measured the neonatal imitation skills and brain electrical activity responses, while infants produced and observed facial gestures. We hypothesized that early social experiences may shape brain activity, as assessed via electroencephalogram suppression in the α band (5-7 Hz in infants, known as the mu rhythm) during action observation, and lead to more proficient imitation skills. Consistent with this hypothesis, the infants reared by their mothers were more likely to imitate lipsmacking (LS) - a natural, affiliative gesture - and exhibited greater mu rhythm desynchronization while viewing LS gestures than the nursery-reared infants. These effects were not found in response to tongue protrusion, a meaningless gesture, or a nonsocial control. These data suggest that socially enriched early experiences in the first days after birth increase brain sensitivity to socially relevant actions.

[1]  S. Suomi,et al.  Delayed Imitation of Lipsmacking Gestures by Infant Rhesus Macaques (Macaca mulatta) , 2011, PloS one.

[2]  N. Fox,et al.  Spectral characteristics of the newborn rhesus macaque EEG reflect functional cortical activity , 2012, Physiology & Behavior.

[3]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Developing a brain specialized for face perception: a converging methods approach. , 2002, Developmental psychobiology.

[4]  S. Jacobson,et al.  Matching behavior in the young infant. , 1979, Child development.

[5]  M. Del Giudice,et al.  Programmed to learn? The ontogeny of mirror neurons. , 2009, Developmental science.

[6]  D. Lewkowicz,et al.  The emergence of multisensory systems through perceptual narrowing , 2009, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[7]  R. Hari Action-perception connection and the cortical mu rhythm. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[8]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Movement-related desynchronization of alpha rhythms is lower in athletes than non-athletes: A high-resolution EEG study , 2010, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[9]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Imitation in Newborn Infants: Exploring the Range of Gestures Imitated and the Underlying Mechanisms. , 1989, Developmental psychology.

[10]  Y. Sugita Face perception in monkeys reared with no exposure to faces , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[11]  N. Fox,et al.  The mirror mechanism and mu rhythm in social development , 2013, Neuroscience Letters.

[12]  M. Rutter,et al.  Developmental catch-up, and deficit, following adoption after severe global early privation. English and Romanian Adoptees (ERA) Study Team. , 1998, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[13]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation. , 2006, Progress in brain research.

[14]  Ross E. Vanderwert,et al.  Distinct EEG Amplitude Suppression to Facial Gestures as Evidence for a Mirror Mechanism in Newborn Monkeys , 2012, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[15]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The Dynamics of Sensorimotor Cortical Oscillations during the Observation of Hand Movements: An EEG Study , 2012, PloS one.

[16]  B. Allison,et al.  The effects of self-movement, observation, and imagination on mu rhythms and readiness potentials (RP's): toward a brain-computer interface (BCI). , 2000, IEEE transactions on rehabilitation engineering : a publication of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society.

[17]  S. Suomi,et al.  Reciprocal Face-to-Face Communication between Rhesus Macaque Mothers and Their Newborn Infants , 2009, Current Biology.

[18]  Kathy Hirsh-Pasek,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[19]  A. Iriki,et al.  Mirror neurons through the lens of epigenetics , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[20]  J. Pineda The functional significance of mu rhythms: Translating “seeing” and “hearing” into “doing” , 2005, Brain Research Reviews.

[21]  B. Röder,et al.  Sensitive periods for the functional specialization of the neural system for human face processing , 2013, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[22]  Lara Bardi,et al.  The processing of social stimuli in early infancy: from faces to biological motion perception. , 2011, Progress in brain research.

[23]  H. Théoret,et al.  EEG evidence for the presence of an action observation–execution matching system in children , 2006, The European journal of neuroscience.

[24]  Mark H Johnson,et al.  Sensitive periods in functional brain development: problems and prospects. , 2005, Developmental psychobiology.

[25]  P. Boyer,et al.  Neuroscience and Biobehavioral , 2022 .

[26]  D. Maestripieri Gestural Communication in Macaques: Usage and Meaning of Nonvocal Signals , 1997 .

[27]  S. Suomi,et al.  Neonatal imitation predicts how infants engage with faces. , 2014, Developmental science.

[28]  C. Umilta,et al.  Face preference at birth. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[29]  C. Nelson,et al.  Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions , 2009, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[30]  N. Fox,et al.  Frequency and topography in monkey electroencephalogram during action observation: possible neural correlates of the mirror neuron system , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  S. Suomi,et al.  Visual attention during neonatal imitation in newborn macaque monkeys. , 2014, Developmental psychobiology.

[32]  Done-Sik Yoo,et al.  Neural correlates related to action observation in expert archers , 2011, Behavioural Brain Research.

[33]  D. Bjorklund The role of immaturity in human development. , 1997 .

[34]  K. Nelson,et al.  Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. , 1989, Scandinavian journal of psychology.

[35]  C. Keysers,et al.  μ-Suppression during Action Observation and Execution Correlates with BOLD in Dorsal Premotor, Inferior Parietal, and SI Cortices , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[36]  S. Cochin,et al.  Perception of motion and qEEG activity in human adults. , 1998, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[37]  Blake W. Johnson,et al.  Mu rhythm modulation during observation of an object-directed grasp. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[38]  D. Bjorklund The role of immaturity in human development. , 1997, Psychological bulletin.

[39]  Hein T. van Schie,et al.  You'll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy , 2008, NeuroImage.

[40]  N. Fox,et al.  Neonatal imitation and an epigenetic account of mirror neuron development. , 2014, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[41]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[42]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[43]  A. Arteche,et al.  The effects of maternal postnatal depression and child sex on academic performance at age 16 years: a developmental approach. , 2010, Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines.

[44]  P. Ferrari,et al.  The mirror neuron system as revealed through neonatal imitation: presence from birth, predictive power and evidence of plasticity , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[45]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  A moment of silence: How the prosodic cues in motherese might assist language learning , 1986 .

[46]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Neural mirroring systems: Exploring the EEG mu rhythm in human infancy , 2011, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  V. Slaughter,et al.  Of hissing snakes and angry voices: human infants are differentially responsive to evolutionary fear-relevant sounds. , 2013, Developmental science.

[48]  S. Suomi,et al.  Interindividual differences in neonatal imitation and the development of action chains in rhesus macaques. , 2009, Child development.

[49]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience How Learning to Shake a Rattle Affects 8-month-old Infants' Perception of the Rattle's Sound: Electrophysiological Evidence for Action-effect Binding in Infancy , 2022 .

[50]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Motor imagery activates primary sensorimotor area in humans , 1997, Neuroscience Letters.

[51]  Ross E. Vanderwert,et al.  Action Experience, More than Observation, Influences Mu Rhythm Desynchronization , 2014, PloS one.

[52]  M. Heil,et al.  Expertise in dance modulates alpha/beta event‐related desynchronization during action observation , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[53]  Margaret C. Moulson,et al.  I spy with my little eye: Typical, daily exposure to faces documented from a first‐person infant perspective , 2013, Developmental psychobiology.

[54]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Neonatal Imitation in Rhesus Macaques , 2006, PLoS biology.