Stable and variable affordances are both automatic and flexible

The mere observation of pictures or words referring to manipulable objects is sufficient to evoke their affordances since objects and their nouns elicit components of appropriate motor programs associated with object interaction. While nobody doubts that objects actually evoke motor information, the degree of automaticity of this activation has been recently disputed. Recent evidence has indeed revealed that affordances activation is flexibly modulated by the task and by the physical and social context. It is therefore crucial to understand whether these results challenge previous evidence showing that motor information is activated independently from the task. The context and the task can indeed act as an early or late filter. We will review recent data consistent with the notion that objects automatically elicit multiple affordances and that top-down processes select among them probably inhibiting motor information that is not consistent with behavior goals. We will therefore argue that automaticity and flexibility of affordances are not in conflict. We will also discuss how language can incorporate affordances showing similarities, but also differences, between the motor information elicited by vision and language. Finally we will show how the distinction between stable and variable affordances can accommodate all these effects.

[1]  Andrea Flumini,et al.  Context and hand posture modulate the neural dynamics of tool–object perception , 2013, Neuropsychologia.

[2]  Steven A. Jax,et al.  Response interference between functional and structural actions linked to the same familiar object , 2010, Cognition.

[3]  F. Binkofski,et al.  Two action systems in the human brain , 2013, Brain and Language.

[4]  Gaetano Tieri,et al.  Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[5]  Giovanni Pezzulo,et al.  How do you hold your mouse? Tracking the compatibility effect between hand posture and stimulus size , 2015, Psychological research.

[6]  Rolf A. Zwaan,et al.  Approach and avoidance as action effects , 2008, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[7]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. , 2007, Human movement science.

[8]  Michael P. Kaschak,et al.  Constructing Meaning: The Role of Affordances and Grammatical Constructions in Sentence Comprehension , 2000 .

[9]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Extending the mirror neuron system model, II: what did I just do? A new role for mirror neurons , 2010, Biological Cybernetics.

[10]  S. Tipper,et al.  Vision-for-action: The effects of object property discrimination and action state on affordance compatibility effects , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[11]  A. Glenberg,et al.  Action-based language: A theory of language acquisition, comprehension, and production , 2012, Cortex.

[12]  L. Wheaton,et al.  I give you a cup, I get a cup: A kinematic study on social intention , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[13]  L. Riggio,et al.  The role of attention in the occurrence of the affordance effect. , 2008, Acta psychologica.

[14]  Serge Thill,et al.  Affordances, Adaptive Tool Use and Grounded Cognition , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[15]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Visual context modulates potentiation of grasp types during semantic object categorization , 2013, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review.

[16]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Learning to use novel objects: A training study on the acquisition of novel action representations , 2011, Consciousness and Cognition.

[17]  Anna M. Borghi,et al.  Action language comprehension , affordances and goals . In , 2011 .

[18]  Michael Chaskalson,et al.  Approach and Avoidance , 2011 .

[19]  L. Barsalou Grounded cognition. , 2008, Annual review of psychology.

[20]  Angelo Cangelosi,et al.  Visual and linguistic cues to graspable objects , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[21]  Sarah H. Creem,et al.  Grasping objects by their handles: a necessary interaction between cognition and action. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[22]  M. Jeannerod The cognitive neuroscience of action , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[23]  Diane Pecher,et al.  No role for motor affordances in visual working memory. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[24]  G. Goldenberg Apraxia and the parietal lobes , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[25]  Monica Maranesi,et al.  Cortical processing of object affordances for self and others' action , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[26]  B. Mesquita,et al.  Adjustment to Chronic Diseases and Terminal Illness Health Psychology : Psychological Adjustment to Chronic Disease , 2006 .

[27]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  Thinking as the control of imagination: a conceptual framework for goal-directed systems , 2009, Psychological research.

[28]  D. Algom,et al.  A rational look at the emotional stroop phenomenon: a generic slowdown, not a stroop effect. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[29]  U. Castiello,et al.  Does the intention to communicate affect action kinematics? , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[30]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Coding of peripersonal space in inferior premotor cortex (area F4). , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[31]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[32]  B. Bahrami,et al.  Coming of age: A review of embodiment and the neuroscience of semantics , 2012, Cortex.

[33]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Social Requests and Social Affordances: How They Affect the Kinematics of Motor Sequences during Interactions between Conspecifics , 2011, PloS one.

[34]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Selectivity for grip type and action goal in macaque inferior parietal and ventral premotor grasping neurons. , 2012, Journal of neurophysiology.

[35]  G. Pezzulo,et al.  When affordances climb into your mind: Advantages of motor simulation in a memory task performed by novice and expert rock climbers , 2010, Brain and Cognition.

[36]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Context effects on the processing of action-relevant object features. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[37]  V. Gallese,et al.  Objects and their nouns in peripersonal space , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[38]  Luigi Cattaneo,et al.  Broken affordances, broken objects: A TMS study , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  Alessandro Livi,et al.  Space-Dependent Representation of Objects and Other's Action in Monkey Ventral Premotor Grasping Neurons , 2014, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[40]  Seth A. Herd,et al.  A Unified Framework for Inhibitory Control Opinion , 2022 .

[41]  Paul Cisek,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis , 2007, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[42]  D. Bub,et al.  Priming of reach and grasp actions by handled objects. , 2011, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  Guy A. Orban,et al.  The neural basis of human tool use , 2014, Front. Psychol..

[44]  Sarah H. Creem-Regehr,et al.  Neural representations of graspable objects: are tools special? , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[45]  B. Hommel,et al.  Directive and incentive functions of affective action consequences: an ideomotor approach , 2015, Psychological research.

[46]  Scott H. Johnson-Frey The neural bases of complex tool use in humans , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[47]  A. Borghi,et al.  Object Affordances Tune Observers' Prior Expectations about Tool-Use Behaviors , 2012, PloS one.

[48]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Parietal Lobe: From Action Organization to Intention Understanding , 2005, Science.

[49]  T. Ziemke,et al.  Theories and computational models of affordance and mirror systems: An integrative review , 2013, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[50]  R. Ellis,et al.  On the relations between seen objects and components of potential actions. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[51]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[52]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[53]  M. Jeannerod The timing of natural prehension movements. , 1984, Journal of motor behavior.

[54]  M. Costantini,et al.  When objects are close to me: Affordances in the peripersonal space , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[55]  G. Buccino,et al.  The role of affordances in inhibition of return , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[56]  George S. Cree,et al.  Evocation of functional and volumetric gestural knowledge by objects and words , 2008, Cognition.

[57]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Object representation in the ventral premotor cortex (area F5) of the monkey. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[58]  Diane Pecher,et al.  Introduction to the Special Topic Embodied and Grounded Cognition , 2011, Front. Psychology.

[59]  D. Parisi,et al.  TRoPICALS : A Computational Embodied Neuroscience Model of Experiments on Compatibility Effects , 2010 .

[60]  Alex Martin,et al.  Representation of Manipulable Man-Made Objects in the Dorsal Stream , 2000, NeuroImage.

[61]  M. Jeannerod The representing brain: Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery , 1994, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[62]  Michael A. Arbib,et al.  Perceptual Structures and Distributed Motor Control , 1981 .

[63]  D. Parisi,et al.  TRoPICALS: a computational embodied neuroscience model of compatibility effects. , 2010, Psychological review.

[64]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Action semantics: A unifying conceptual framework for the selective use of multimodal and modality-specific object knowledge. , 2014, Physics of life reviews.

[65]  A. Ohman,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[66]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[67]  A. Flykt,et al.  Emotion drives attention: detecting the snake in the grass. , 2001 .

[68]  Sinan Kalkan,et al.  Human and robotics hands grasping danger , 2012, The 2012 International Joint Conference on Neural Networks (IJCNN).

[69]  Glyn W Humphreys,et al.  Neuropsychological evidence for visual- and motor-based affordance: effects of reference frame and object-hand congruence. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[70]  A. Chemero Radical Embodied Cognitive Science , 2009 .

[71]  Garry Young,et al.  Are different affordances subserved by different neural pathways? , 2006, Brain and Cognition.

[72]  A. Borghi Language comprehension: Action, affordances and goals , 2012 .

[73]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The mirror-neuron system. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[74]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Emotive concept nouns and motor responses: Attraction or repulsion? , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[75]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Cortex special issue: Language and the motor system , 2012, Cortex.

[76]  Lucia Riggio,et al.  Sentence comprehension and simulation of object temporary, canonical and stable affordances , 2009, Brain Research.

[77]  Lewis A. Wheaton,et al.  The Neuroscience of Storing and Molding Tool Action Concepts: How “Plastic” is Grounded Cognition? , 2010, Front. Psychology.

[78]  R. Ellis,et al.  The potentiation of grasp types during visual object categorization , 2001 .

[79]  V. Gallese Mirror neurons and the social nature of language: The neural exploitation hypothesis , 2008, Social neuroscience.

[80]  S. Chaiken,et al.  Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- Tin Chen, Bargh / Consequences of Automatic Evaluation Immediate Behavioral Predispositions to Approach or Avoid the Stimulus , 2022 .

[81]  M. Goodale,et al.  Separate visual pathways for perception and action , 1992, Trends in Neurosciences.

[82]  R. Ellis,et al.  Micro-affordance: the potentiation of components of action by seen objects. , 2000, British journal of psychology.

[83]  L. Buxbaum,et al.  Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems , 2010, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.

[84]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Functional properties of grasping-related neurons in the ventral premotor area F5 of the macaque monkey. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[85]  John Bridgeman,et al.  Bodies and other visual objects: the dialectics of reaching toward objects , 2013, Psychological research.

[86]  M. Brett,et al.  Actions Speak Louder Than Functions: The Importance of Manipulability and Action in Tool Representation , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[87]  L. Wheaton,et al.  One hand, two objects: Emergence of affordance in contexts , 2012, Brain and Cognition.

[88]  L. Fogassi,et al.  Functional properties of grasping-related neurons in the dorsal premotor area F2 of the macaque monkey. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[89]  M. Costantini,et al.  Author ' s personal copy Short Communication Which body for embodied cognition ? Affordance and language within actual and perceived reaching space , 2012 .

[90]  A. Cangelosi,et al.  How affordances associated with a distractor object affect compatibility effects: A study with the computational model TRoPICALS , 2013, Psychological research.

[91]  M. Arbib,et al.  Grasping objects: the cortical mechanisms of visuomotor transformation , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[92]  Olaf B. Paulson,et al.  When Action Turns into Words. Activation of Motor-Based Knowledge during Categorization of Manipulable Objects , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[93]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[94]  Andrea Moro,et al.  Negation in the brain: Modulating action representations , 2008, NeuroImage.

[95]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[96]  Giorgia Committeri,et al.  Ready Both to Your and to My Hands: Mapping the Action Space of Others , 2011, PloS one.

[97]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Two different streams form the dorsal visual system: anatomy and functions , 2003, Experimental Brain Research.

[98]  Anatomical and functional subdivisions of inferior area 6 in macaque monkey , 1996 .

[99]  D. Mirman,et al.  Incidental and context-responsive activation of structure- and function-based action features during object identification. , 2013, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[100]  Alfred B. Yu,et al.  Limits on action priming by pictures of objects. , 2014, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[101]  F. Osiurak Apraxia of tool use is not a matter of affordances , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..

[102]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Premotor Cortex Activation during Observation and Naming of Familiar Tools , 1997, NeuroImage.

[103]  Sergio E Chaigneau,et al.  Assessing the causal structure of function. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[104]  L. Fadiga,et al.  Active perception: sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[105]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Perceiving object dangerousness: an escape from pain? , 2013, Experimental Brain Research.

[106]  V. Gallese The "Conscious" Dorsal Stream: Embodied Simulation and its Role in Space and Action Conscious Awareness , 2007 .

[107]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  A distributed left hemisphere network active during planning of everyday tool use skills. , 2004, Cerebral cortex.

[108]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Grasping the Intentions of Others with One's Own Mirror Neuron System , 2005, PLoS biology.

[109]  G. Humphreys,et al.  The paired-object affordance effect. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[110]  R. Knight,et al.  Prefrontal cortex regulates inhibition and excitation in distributed neural networks. , 1999, Acta psychologica.

[111]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Simon-Like and Functional Affordance Effects with Tools: The Effects of Object Perceptual Discrimination and Object Action State , 2010, Quarterly journal of experimental psychology.

[112]  Annabelle Blangero,et al.  Got it! Understanding the concept of a tool , 2010, NeuroImage.

[113]  M. Jeannerod Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects , 1981 .