Children Change Their Answers in Response to Neutral Follow‐Up Questions by a Knowledgeable Asker
暂无分享,去创建一个
Patrick Shafto | Elizabeth Bonawitz | Yue Yu | Sophie Bridgers | Aaron Gonzalez | Patrick Shafto | E. Bonawitz | Sophie Bridgers | Yue Yu | Aaron Gonzalez
[1] T. Griffiths,et al. NAÏVE THEORIES, AMBIGUOUS EVIDENCE Page 1 Running head: NAÏVE THEORIES AND AMBIGUOUS EVIDENCE Can being scared make your tummy ache? Naive theories, ambiguous evidence and preschoolers’ causal inferences , 2007 .
[2] C. Glymour,et al. Preschool children learn about causal structure from conditional interventions. , 2007, Developmental science.
[3] A. Gopnik,et al. Children's causal inferences from conflicting testimony and observations. , 2016, Developmental psychology.
[4] L. Schulz,et al. Children balance theories and evidence in exploration, explanation, and learning , 2012, Cognitive Psychology.
[5] P. Harris,et al. Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. , 2005, Child development.
[6] V. Jaswal,et al. Adults Don't Always Know Best , 2006, Psychological science.
[7] E. Loftus,et al. The influence of the form of the question on the eyewitness testimony of preschool children , 1976 .
[8] T. Griffiths,et al. Can being scared cause tummy aches? Naive theories, ambiguous evidence, and preschoolers' causal inferences. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[9] D. Poole,et al. Effects of question repetition on the eyewitness testimony of children and adults. , 1991 .
[10] Noah D. Goodman,et al. Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play , 2011, Cognition.
[11] D. Sobel,et al. Children monitor individuals' expertise for word learning. , 2010, Child development.
[12] D. Poole,et al. Tell me again and again: Stability and change in the repeated testimonies of children and adults. , 1995 .
[13] P. Howie,et al. ‘Undesirable’ and ‘desirable’ shifts in children's responses to repeated questions: age differences in the effect of providing a rationale for repetition , 2004 .
[14] J. Pearl. Causality: Models, Reasoning and Inference , 2000 .
[15] Michael C. Frank,et al. Children's social referencing reflects sensitivity to graded uncertainty , 2017, CogSci.
[16] A. Gopnik,et al. Causal learning across domains. , 2004, Developmental psychology.
[17] Yvonne Jaeger. Memory And Testimony In The Child Witness , 2016 .
[18] A. Gopnik,et al. Conditional probability versus spatial contiguity in causal learning: Preschoolers use new contingency evidence to overcome prior spatial assumptions. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[19] Yi Ting Huang,et al. Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics–pragmatics interface , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[20] J. Snedeker,et al. Semantic meaning and pragmatic interpretation in 5-year-olds: evidence from real-time spoken language comprehension. , 2009, Developmental psychology.
[21] I. Noveck. When children are more logical than adults: experimental investigations of scalar implicature , 2001, Cognition.
[22] Kathleen H Corriveau,et al. Young children's selective trust in informants , 2011, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[23] Baxter S. Eaves,et al. Epistemic trust: modeling children's reasoning about others' knowledge and intent. , 2012, Developmental science.
[24] Josef Perner,et al. Early Deception and the Child's Theory of Mind: False Trails and Genuine Markers , 1991 .
[25] Noah D. Goodman,et al. The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery , 2011, Cognition.
[26] Paul L. Harris,et al. The Ontogenesis of Trust , 2004 .
[27] E. Markman,et al. Preschoolers use intentional and pedagogical cues to guide inductive inferences and exploration. , 2012, Child development.
[28] A. Papafragou,et al. Scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics–pragmatics interface , 2003, Cognition.
[29] L. Schulz,et al. 16-Month-Olds Rationally Infer Causes of Failed Actions , 2011, Science.
[30] A. Gopnik,et al. Children’s imitation of causal action sequences is influenced by statistical and pedagogical evidence , 2011, Cognition.
[31] M. Blades,et al. The effect of repeated questioning on children's accuracy and consistency in eyewitness testimony , 2009 .
[32] M. Lamb,et al. Do we need to rethink guidance on repeated interviews , 2010 .
[33] J. Tenenbaum,et al. Infants consider both the sample and the sampling process in inductive generalization , 2010, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[34] Debra A. Poole,et al. Two years later: Effect of question repetition and retention interval on the eyewitness testimony of children and adults. , 1993 .
[35] Tom Burr,et al. Causation, Prediction, and Search , 2003, Technometrics.
[36] L. Brown,et al. Interval Estimation for a Binomial Proportion , 2001 .
[37] H. Diessel. Corpus linguistics and language acquisition , 2007 .
[38] P. Harris,et al. Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. , 2007, Developmental psychology.
[39] Siobhan Chapman. Logic and Conversation , 2005 .
[40] Michael C. Frank,et al. Children's Pragmatic Inferences as a Route for Learning About the World. , 2016, Child Development.
[41] H. Wellman,et al. Young Children Use Statistical Sampling to Infer the Preferences of Other People , 2010, Psychological science.
[42] W. Thompson,et al. Do Jurors “Know” What Isn’t So About Child Witnesses? , 2005, Law and human behavior.
[43] David Barner,et al. Finding one’s meaning: A test of the relation between quantifiers and integers in language development , 2009, Cognitive Psychology.
[44] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding and sharing intentions , 2005 .
[45] M. Tomasello,et al. Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition , 2005, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.