Action‐semantic and syntactic deficits in subjects at risk for Huntington's disease

Frontostriatal networks play critical roles in grounding action semantics and syntactic skills. Indeed, their atrophy distinctively disrupts both domains, as observed in patients with Huntington's disease (HD) and Parkinson's disease, even during early disease stages. However, frontostriatal degeneration in these conditions may begin up to 15 years before the onset of clinical symptoms, opening avenues for pre-clinical detection via sensitive tasks. Such a mission is particularly critical in HD, given that patients' children have 50% chances of inheriting the disease. Against this background, we assessed whether deficits in the above-mentioned domains emerge in subjects at risk to develop HD. We administered tasks tapping action semantics, object semantics, and two forms of syntactic processing to 18 patients with HD, 19 asymptomatic first-degree relatives, and sociodemographically matched controls for each group. The patients evinced significant deficits in all tasks, but only those in the two target domains were independent of overall cognitive state. More crucially, relative to controls, the asymptomatic relatives were selectively impaired in action semantics and in the more complex syntactic task, with both patterns emerging irrespective of the subjects' overall cognitive state. Our findings highlight the relevance of these dysfunctions as potential prodromal biomarkers of HD. Moreover, they offer theoretical insights into the differential contributions of frontostriatal hubs to both domains while paving the way for innovations in diagnostic procedures.

[1]  E. Ray Dorsey,et al.  Characterization of a Large Group of Individuals with Huntington Disease and Their Relatives Enrolled in the COHORT Study , 2012, PloS one.

[2]  P. Remy,et al.  Language processing within the striatum: evidence from a PET correlation study in Huntington's disease. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[3]  P. Panegyres,et al.  The neurology and natural history of patients with indeterminate CAG repeat length mutations of the Huntington disease gene , 2011, Journal of the Neurological Sciences.

[4]  M. Desmurget,et al.  Basal ganglia contributions to motor control: a vigorous tutor , 2010, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[5]  R. D Rogers,et al.  Impaired planning but intact decision making in early Huntington’s disease: implications for specific fronto-striatal pathology , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[6]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Hands typing what hands do: Action–semantic integration dynamics throughout written verb production , 2016, Cognition.

[7]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Network topology and functional connectivity disturbances precede the onset of Huntington's disease. , 2015, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  J. Hodges,et al.  Kissing and dancing—a test to distinguish the lexical and conceptual contributions to noun/verb and action/object dissociation. Preliminary results in patients with frontotemporal dementia , 2003, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[9]  A. D. Friederici,et al.  The neurobiological nature of syntactic hierarchies , 2017, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[10]  S. Grillner,et al.  Mechanisms for selection of basic motor programs – roles for the striatum and pallidum , 2005, Trends in Neurosciences.

[11]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  The Road Less Traveled: Alternative Pathways for Action-Verb Processing in Parkinson's Disease. , 2016, Journal of Alzheimer's disease : JAD.

[12]  M. Saraiva-Pereira,et al.  Genetic aspects of Huntington's disease in Latin America. A systematic review , 2016, Clinical genetics.

[13]  S. Baez,et al.  Impairments in negative emotion recognition and empathy for pain in Huntington's disease families , 2015, Neuropsychologia.

[14]  T. Shallice,et al.  Supervisory and Routine Processes in Noun and Verb Generation in Nondemented Patients with Parkinson's Disease , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  S. Cappa,et al.  Nouns and verbs in the brain: A review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies , 2011, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[16]  S. Baez,et al.  Your misery is no longer my pleasure: Reduced schadenfreude in Huntington's disease families , 2016, Cortex.

[17]  Daniel Weintraub,et al.  Montreal Cognitive Assessment Performance in Patients with Parkinson's Disease with “Normal” Global Cognition According to Mini‐Mental State Examination Score , 2009, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[18]  Rutvik H. Desai,et al.  Parkinson’s disease disrupts both automatic and controlled processing of action verbs , 2013, Brain and Language.

[19]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  The role of the striatum in sentence processing: Evidence from a priming study in early stages of Huntington's disease , 2008, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  T. Anderson,et al.  The MoCA , 2010, Neurology.

[21]  M. Grossman,et al.  Attentional resource and processing speed limitations during sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Brain and Language.

[22]  S. Cappa,et al.  Action and object naming in Parkinson's disease without dementia , 2007, European journal of neurology.

[23]  Byron Bernal,et al.  How Localized are Language Brain Areas? A Review of Brodmann Areas Involvement in Oral Language. , 2016, Archives of clinical neuropsychology : the official journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists.

[24]  T. Bak,et al.  The neuroscience of action semantics in neurodegenerative brain diseases. , 2013, Current opinion in neurology.

[25]  F. Manes,et al.  Action-verb processing in Parkinson’s disease: new pathways for motor–language coupling , 2013, Brain Structure and Function.

[26]  Hiroko Nakano,et al.  The roles of sequencing and verbal working memory in sentence comprehension deficits in Parkinson’s disease , 2006, Brain and Language.

[27]  Bernard Ravina,et al.  The montreal cognitive assessment as a screening tool for cognitive impairment in Parkinson's disease , 2008, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[28]  M. Sigman,et al.  The man who feels two hearts: the different pathways of interoception. , 2014, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[29]  J. Gee,et al.  Grammatical and resource components of sentence processing in Parkinson’s disease , 2003, Neurology.

[30]  S. Fahn,et al.  Huntington disease , 1979, Neurology.

[31]  A. Siderowf,et al.  Validity of the MoCA and MMSE in the detection of MCI and dementia in Parkinson disease , 2009, Neurology.

[32]  Lucas Sedeño,et al.  Insular networks for emotional processing and social cognition: Comparison of two case reports with either cortical or subcortical involvement , 2013, Cortex.

[33]  G. Lakoff,et al.  The Brain's concepts: the role of the Sensory-motor system in conceptual knowledge , 2005, Cognitive neuropsychology.

[34]  H. Duffau,et al.  Cortico-striatal language pathways dynamically adjust for syntactic complexity: A computational study , 2017, Brain and Language.

[35]  G. Gigli,et al.  Greater syntactic impairments in native language in bilingual Parkinsonian patients , 2004, Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry.

[36]  I. T. Draper THE ASSESSMENT OF APHASIA AND RELATED DISORDERS , 1973 .

[37]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  The cerebellum and embodied semantics: evidence from a case of genetic ataxia due to STUB1 mutations , 2016, Journal of Medical Genetics.

[38]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  Motor–Language Coupling in Huntington’s Disease Families , 2014, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[39]  Paul H. Garthwaite,et al.  Single-case research in neuropsychology: A comparison of five forms of t-test for comparing a case to controls , 2012, Cortex.

[40]  J. Hodges,et al.  The effects of motor neurone disease on language: Further evidence , 2004, Brain and Language.

[41]  Carol Brayne,et al.  Montreal Cognitive Assessment for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease and other dementias. , 2015, The Cochrane database of systematic reviews.

[42]  J. Cummings,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A Brief Screening Tool For Mild Cognitive Impairment , 2005, Journal of the American Geriatrics Society.

[43]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Brain mechanisms linking language and action , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[44]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  The cortical localization of the lexicons. Positron emission tomography evidence. , 1992, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[45]  C. Restrepo,et al.  Genetics and Genomic Medicine in Colombia , 2015, Molecular genetics & genomic medicine.

[46]  L. Barsalou,et al.  Whither structured representation? , 1999, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[47]  Paul H. Garthwaite,et al.  On comparing a single case with a control sample: An alternative perspective , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[48]  E. Růžička,et al.  Validity of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment in the Detection of Cognitive Dysfunction in Huntington's Disease , 2013, Applied neuropsychology. Adult.

[49]  M. Ullman Contributions of memory circuits to language: the declarative/procedural model , 2004, Cognition.

[50]  G. Fitzmaurice,et al.  Rapid cognitive screening of patients with substance use disorders. , 2009, Experimental and clinical psychopharmacology.

[51]  R. de Diego-Balaguer,et al.  Striatal degeneration impairs language learning: evidence from Huntington's disease. , 2008, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[52]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Language deficits as a preclinical window into Parkinson’s disease: Evidence from asymptomatic parkin and dardarin mutation carriers , 2016 .

[53]  Elmar Nöth,et al.  How language flows when movements don’t: An automated analysis of spontaneous discourse in Parkinson’s disease , 2016, Brain and Language.

[54]  P. Stanzione,et al.  Grammar improvement following deep brain stimulation of the subthalamic and the pedunculopontine nuclei in advanced Parkinson's disease: a pilot study. , 2009, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[55]  Dominique Sportiche,et al.  The role of the striatum in sentence processing: Disentangling syntax from working memory in Huntington’s disease , 2012, Neuropsychologia.

[56]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Comparing a single case to a control sample: Testing for neuropsychological deficits and dissociations in the presence of covariates , 2011, Cortex.

[57]  Philip Lieberman,et al.  Speech production, syntax comprehension, and cognitive deficits in Parkinson's disease , 1992, Brain and Language.

[58]  F. Manes,et al.  How embodied is action language? Neurological evidence from motor diseases , 2014, Cognition.

[59]  L. Hartelius,et al.  Comprehension of complex discourse in different stages of Huntington's disease. , 2010, International journal of language & communication disorders.

[60]  M. Panas,et al.  Low plasma total cholesterol in patients with Huntington's disease and first-degree relatives. , 2008, Molecular genetics and metabolism.

[61]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Unspeakable motion: Selective action-verb impairments in Parkinson’s disease patients without mild cognitive impairment , 2017, Brain and Language.

[62]  P. Garthwaite,et al.  Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences , 2002, Neuropsychologia.

[63]  D. Copland,et al.  A comparison of picture description abilities in individuals with vascular subcortical lesions and Huntington's Disease. , 2006, Journal of communication disorders.

[64]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  A Lesion-Proof Brain? Multidimensional Sensorimotor, Cognitive, and Socio-Affective Preservation Despite Extensive Damage in a Stroke Patient , 2017, Front. Aging Neurosci..

[65]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Syntax, action verbs, action semantics, and object semantics in Parkinson's disease: Dissociability, progression, and executive influences , 2015, Cortex.

[66]  E. Bézard,et al.  Initial clinical manifestations of Parkinson's disease: features and pathophysiological mechanisms , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[67]  C. Gillberg,et al.  Looking Backward to Move Forward: Early Detection of Neurodegenerative Disorders , 2003 .

[68]  Nick C Fox,et al.  Biological and clinical manifestations of Huntington's disease in the longitudinal TRACK-HD study: cross-sectional analysis of baseline data , 2009, The Lancet Neurology.

[69]  Emmanuel Dupoux,et al.  The Role of the Striatum in Processing Language Rules: Evidence from Word Perception in Huntington's Disease , 2006, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[70]  H. Karnath,et al.  Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[71]  J. Wixted,et al.  A comparison of two brief screening measures of cognitive impairment in Huntington's disease , 2010, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[72]  Agustín Ibáñez,et al.  A touch with words: Dynamic synergies between manual actions and language , 2016, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[73]  Nagaendran Kandiah,et al.  Montreal Cognitive Assessment for the screening and prediction of cognitive decline in early Parkinson's disease. , 2014, Parkinsonism & related disorders.

[74]  R. Hichwa,et al.  Longitudinal neuropsychological and genetic linkage analysis of persons at risk for Huntington's disease. , 1995, Archives of neurology.

[75]  Egberto Reis Barbosa,et al.  Language impairment in Huntington's disease. , 2012, Arquivos de neuro-psiquiatria.

[76]  Roberto Eleopra,et al.  To move or not to move: Subthalamic deep brain stimulation effects on implicit motor simulation , 2014, Brain Research.

[77]  Albert Postma,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) is Superior to the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) in Detection of Korsakoff’s Syndrome , 2014, The Clinical neuropsychologist.

[78]  M. Gentilucci,et al.  Spatiotemporal dynamics during processing of abstract and concrete verbs: An ERP study , 2014, Neuropsychologia.

[79]  Jane S. Paulsen,et al.  Unified Huntington's disease rating scale: Reliability and consistency , 1996, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[80]  M. Ullman A neurocognitive perspective on language: The declarative/procedural model , 2001, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[81]  S. Leurgans,et al.  The Montreal Cognitive Assessment as a screening tool for cognitive dysfunction in Huntington's disease , 2010, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[82]  Matthew Menear,et al.  Brain imaging and cognitive dysfunctions in Huntington's disease. , 2006, Journal of psychiatry & neuroscience : JPN.

[83]  A. Ibáñez,et al.  Words in motion: Motor-language coupling in Parkinson’s disease , 2014 .

[84]  D. C. Howell,et al.  Regression equations in clinical neuropsychology: an evaluation of statistical methods for comparing predicted and obtained scores. , 1998, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[85]  Paul E. Gilbert,et al.  Usefulness of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) in Huntington's disease , 2013, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[86]  N. Jetté,et al.  The incidence and prevalence of Huntington's disease: A systematic review and meta‐analysis , 2012, Movement disorders : official journal of the Movement Disorder Society.

[87]  Thomas Straube,et al.  No impairment of recognition and experience of disgust in a patient with a right-hemispheric lesion of the insula and basal ganglia , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[88]  A. Graybiel,et al.  Role of [corrected] nigrostriatal dopamine system in learning to perform sequential motor tasks in a predictive manner. , 1999, Journal of neurophysiology.

[89]  J. Doyon,et al.  Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related brain structures to motor learning , 2009, Behavioural Brain Research.

[90]  A. Ibanez,et al.  Motor-language coupling: Direct evidence from early Parkinson’s disease and intracranial cortical recordings , 2013, Cortex.