Teaching by Listening: The Importance of Adult-Child Conversations to Language Development

OBJECTIVE: To test the independent association of adult language input, television viewing, and adult-child conversations on language acquisition among infants and toddlers. METHODS: Two hundred seventy-five families of children aged 2 to 48 months who were representative of the US census were enrolled in a cross-sectional study of the home language environment and child language development (phase 1). Of these, a representative sample of 71 families continued for a longitudinal assessment over 18 months (phase 2). In the cross-sectional sample, language development scores were regressed on adult word count, television viewing, and adult-child conversations, controlling for socioeconomic attributes. In the longitudinal sample, phase 2 language development scores were regressed on phase 1 language development, as well as phase 1 adult word count, television viewing, and adult-child conversations, controlling for socioeconomic attributes. RESULTS: In fully adjusted regressions, the effects of adult word count were significant when included alone but were partially mediated by adult-child conversations. Television viewing when included alone was significant and negative but was fully mediated by the inclusion of adult-child conversations. Adult-child conversations were significant when included alone and retained both significance and magnitude when adult word count and television exposure were included. CONCLUSIONS: Television exposure is not independently associated with child language development when adult-child conversations are controlled. Adult-child conversations are robustly associated with healthy language development. Parents should be encouraged not merely to provide language input to their children through reading or storytelling, but also to engage their children in two-sided conversations.

[1]  Umit Yapanel,et al.  Reliability of the LENA Language Environment Analysis System in Young Children’s Natural Home Environment , 2009 .

[2]  Dongxin Xu,et al.  The LENA , 2009 .

[3]  Ted Ruffman,et al.  Stepping stones to others' minds: maternal talk relates to child mental state language and emotion understanding at 15, 24, and 33 months. , 2008, Child development.

[4]  Dimitri A. Christakis,et al.  Associations Between Content Types of Early Media Exposure and Subsequent Attentional Problems , 2007, Pediatrics.

[5]  Rachel Barr,et al.  Age-related changes in deferred imitation from television by 6- to 18-month-olds. , 2007, Developmental science.

[6]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Associations between media viewing and language development in children under age 2 years. , 2007, The Journal of pediatrics.

[7]  M. Krcmar,et al.  Can Toddlers Learn Vocabulary from Television? An Experimental Approach , 2007 .

[8]  Shetal I. Shah Interpretation of maternal influenza study should not discourage immunization efforts. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[9]  Masako Tanimura,et al.  Television viewing, reduced parental utterance, and delayed speech development in infants and young children. , 2007, Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine.

[10]  M. Arterberry,et al.  Early attention and literacy experiences predict adaptive communication , 2007 .

[11]  Andrew N Meltzoff,et al.  Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. , 2007, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[12]  L. Epstein,et al.  Relationship between parental estimate and an objective measure of child television watching , 2006, The international journal of behavioral nutrition and physical activity.

[13]  Dimitri A Christakis,et al.  Children's television viewing and cognitive outcomes: a longitudinal analysis of national data. , 2005, Archives of pediatrics & adolescent medicine.

[14]  J. Singer,et al.  Maternal correlates of growth in toddler vocabulary production in low-income families. , 2005, Child development.

[15]  Daniel R. Anderson,et al.  Television and Very Young Children , 2005 .

[16]  S. Suter Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children , 2005, European Journal of Pediatrics.

[17]  Theodore A. Walls,et al.  More evidence for reach out and read: a home-based study. , 2004, Pediatrics.

[18]  V. Rideout,et al.  Zero to Six: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers and Preschoolers , 2003 .

[19]  E. Clark,et al.  Adult reformulations of child errors as negative evidence , 2003, Journal of Child Language.

[20]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Foreign-language experience in infancy: Effects of short-term exposure and social interaction on phonetic learning , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[21]  Charles Fernyhough,et al.  Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. , 2002, Child development.

[22]  Iman Sharif,et al.  Exposure to Reach Out and Read and vocabulary outcomes in inner city preschoolers. , 2002, Journal of the National Medical Association.

[23]  Ronda M. Scantlin,et al.  The relations of early television viewing to school readiness and vocabulary of children from low-income families: the early window project. , 2001, Child development.

[24]  A. Sandler The impact of a clinic-based literacy intervention on language development in inner-city preschool children , 2001 .

[25]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Maternal responsiveness and children's achievement of language milestones. , 2001, Child development.

[26]  N. Akhtar,et al.  Learning words through overhearing. , 2001, Child development.

[27]  B. Dreyer,et al.  The impact of a clinic-based literacy intervention on language development in inner-city preschool children. , 2001, Pediatrics.

[28]  D. Phillips,et al.  From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development , 2000 .

[29]  Monique Sénéchal,et al.  A book reading intervention with preschool children who have limited vocabularies: the benefits of regular reading and dialogic reading , 2000 .

[30]  Dina L. G. Borzekowski,et al.  Viewing the viewers: Ten video cases of children's television viewing behaviors , 1999 .

[31]  Rebecca J. Panagos Meaningful Differences in the Everyday Experience of Young American Children , 1998 .

[32]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Predicting variation in the timing of language milestones in the second year: an events history approach , 1998, Journal of Child Language.

[33]  M. Bornstein,et al.  Vocabulary competence in early childhood: measurement, latent construct, and predictive validity. , 1998, Child development.

[34]  G. Whitehurst,et al.  Child development and emergent literacy. , 1998, Child development.

[35]  J. Huttenlocher,et al.  Language input and language growth. , 1998, Preventive medicine.

[36]  M. Sénéchal,et al.  The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers' acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary , 1997, Journal of Child Language.

[37]  Charles Fernyhough,et al.  The dialogic mind: A dialogic approach to the higher mental functions , 1996 .

[38]  Christopher J. Lonigan,et al.  Accelerating language development through picture book reading: replication and extension to a videotape training format , 1994 .

[39]  A. Bryk,et al.  Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. , 1991 .

[40]  E. Lorch,et al.  Estimates of young children's time with television: a methodological comparison of parent reports with time-lapse video home observation. , 1985, Child development.