The easy-to-hard effect in human (Homo sapiens) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) auditory identification.

The authors examined whether progressively training humans and rats to perform a difficult auditory identification task led to larger improvements than extensive training with highly similar sounds (the easy-to-hard effect). Practice improved humans' ability to distinguish sounds regardless of the training regimen. However, progressively trained subjects were more accurate and showed more generalization, despite significantly less training with the stimuli that were the most difficult to distinguish. Rats showed less capacity to improve with practice but still benefited from progressive training. These findings indicate that transitioning from an easier to a more difficult task during training can facilitate, and in some cases may be essential for, auditory perceptual learning. The results are not predicted by an explanation that assumes interaction of generalized excitation and inhibition but are consistent with a hierarchical account of perceptual learning in which the representational precision required to distinguish stimuli determines the mechanisms engaged during learning.

[1]  J. Gibson Perception as a function of stimulation , 1959 .

[2]  U. Karmarkar,et al.  Temporal specificity of perceptual learning in an auditory discrimination task. , 2003, Learning & memory.

[3]  Y Sakurai,et al.  Involvement of auditory cortical and hippocampal neurons in auditory working memory and reference memory in the rat , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[4]  G. Gerstein,et al.  Reorganization in awake rat auditory cortex by local microstimulation and its effect on frequency-discrimination behavior. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Me Bitterman,et al.  Transfer along a continuum in the discriminative learning of honeybees , 1990 .

[6]  E. Gibson,et al.  The Psychology of Reading , 1977 .

[7]  I. Mclaren,et al.  Transfer Along a Continuum: Differentiation or Association? , 2000 .

[8]  S. Hochstein,et al.  Task difficulty and the specificity of perceptual learning , 1997, Nature.

[9]  A. Karni,et al.  The time course of learning a visual skill , 1993, Nature.

[10]  M. Gluck,et al.  Basal forebrain stimulation changes cortical sensitivities to complex sound , 2001, Neuroreport.

[11]  N. Kraus,et al.  Auditory training induces asymmetrical changes in cortical neural activity. , 2002, Journal of speech, language, and hearing research : JSLHR.

[12]  Koch Sigmund Ed,et al.  Psychology: A Study of A Science , 1962 .

[13]  E. Gibson,et al.  The effect of prolonged exposure to visually presented patterns on learning to discriminate them. , 1956, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[14]  Michael Brecht,et al.  Map Plasticity in Somatosensory Cortex , 2005, Science.

[15]  E. Wasserman,et al.  Comparative cognition : experimental explorations of animal intelligence , 2009 .

[16]  John T. Wixted Signal Detection Theory , 2005 .

[17]  Geoffrey Hall Perceptual and associative learning , 1991 .

[18]  D. M. Green,et al.  Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .

[19]  H. Kawaguchi,et al.  [Dynamics and histological study of radiation effects on vascular anastomosis]. , 1969, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[20]  I. Mclaren,et al.  Representation and Discrimination on an Artificial Dimension , 2003, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. B, Comparative and physiological psychology.

[21]  F. Logan Transfer of discrimination. , 1966, Journal of experimental psychology.

[22]  D. Buonomano,et al.  Cortical plasticity: from synapses to maps. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.

[23]  D. Moore,et al.  Early and rapid perceptual learning , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[24]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Optimizing sound features for cortical neurons. , 1998, Science.

[25]  Fetterman Jg Dimensions of stimulus complexity. , 1996 .

[26]  John D. Bransford,et al.  Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing , 1977 .

[27]  J. Rauschecker Parallel Processing in the Auditory Cortex of Primates , 1998, Audiology and Neurotology.

[28]  Anne K. Churchland,et al.  Neural correlates of instrumental learning in primary auditory cortex , 2002, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[29]  E. Mercado,et al.  Auditory categorization of complex sounds by rats (Rattus norvegicus). , 2005, Journal of comparative psychology.

[30]  E. Mercado,et al.  Changes in NMDA receptor expression in auditory cortex after learning , 2005, Neuroscience Letters.

[31]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cortical responses in rats predict perceptual sensitivities to complex sounds. , 2005, Behavioral neuroscience.

[32]  N. Mackintosh SELECTIVE ATTENTION IN ANIMAL DISCRIMINATION LEARNING. , 1965, Psychological bulletin.

[33]  T Poggio,et al.  Fast perceptual learning in visual hyperacuity. , 1991, Science.

[34]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  Mechanisms of animal discrimination learning , 1971 .

[35]  N. Weinberger Specific long-term memory traces in primary auditory cortex , 2004, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[37]  M. Cohen,et al.  Effects of categorization and discrimination training on auditory perceptual space. , 1999, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[38]  Kenneth I Forster,et al.  DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy , 2003, Behavior research methods, instruments, & computers : a journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc.

[39]  N. Miller,et al.  Social Learning and Imitation , 1942 .

[40]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Success and failure in teaching the [r]-[l] contrast to Japanese adults: Tests of a Hebbian model of plasticity and stabilization in spoken language perception , 2002, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[41]  N. Mackintosh,et al.  An analysis of transfer along a continuum. , 1970, Canadian journal of psychology.

[42]  N. Mackintosh A Theory of Attention: Variations in the Associability of Stimuli with Reinforcement , 1975 .

[43]  E. Viding,et al.  Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. , 2004, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[44]  Shaul Hochstein,et al.  The spread of attention and learning in feature search: effects of target distribution and task difficulty , 2000, Vision Research.

[45]  John R. Anderson,et al.  Cognitive Tutors: Lessons Learned , 1995 .

[46]  G. Marsh An evaluation of three explanations for the transfer of discrimination effect. , 1969 .

[47]  R A BAKER,et al.  Discrimination transfer along a pitch continuum. , 1954, Journal of experimental psychology.

[48]  Robert L. Goldstone Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[49]  R. Weisman,et al.  Pitch discrimination in field- and isolation-reared black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). , 1997 .

[50]  D. Lawrence,et al.  The transfer of a discrimination along a continuum. , 1952, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.

[51]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  Automatization and training in visual search. , 1992, The American journal of psychology.

[52]  M. Gluck,et al.  Spectrotemporal sensitivities in rat auditory cortical neurons , 2001, Hearing Research.

[53]  M. Merzenich,et al.  Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[54]  Steven L. Miller,et al.  Temporal Processing Deficits of Language-Learning Impaired Children Ameliorated by Training , 1996, Science.

[55]  G A Orban,et al.  Interocular transfer in perceptual learning of a pop-out discrimination task. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[56]  M Ahissar,et al.  Task difficulty and visual hierarchy: Counter-streams in sensory processing and perceptual learning , 1997 .

[57]  D. Norman Learning and Memory , 1982 .