The learning brain: lessons for education: a précis.

This book highlights the importance of anchoring education in an evidence base derived from neuroscience. For far too long has the brain been neglected in discussions on education and often information about neuroscientific research is not easy to access. Our aim was to provide a source book that conveys the excitement of neuroscience research that is relevant to learning and education. This research has largely, but not exclusively, been carried out using neuroimaging methods in the past decade or so, ranging from investigations of brain structure and function in dyslexia and dyscalculia to investigations of the changes in the hippocampus of London taxi drivers. To speak to teachers who might not have scientific backgrounds, we have tried to use nontechnical language as far as possible and have provided an appendix illustrating the main methods and techniques currently used and a glossary, defining terms from Acetylcholine, Action Potentials and ADHD to White Matter, Word Form Area and Working Memory. We start with the idea that the brain has evolved to educate and to be educated, often instinctively and effortlessly. We believe that understanding the brain mechanisms that underlie learning and teaching could transform educational strategies and enable us to design educational programmes that optimize learning for people of all ages and of all needs. For this reason the first two-thirds of the book follows a developmental framework. The rest of the book focuses on learning in the brain at all ages. There is a vast amount brain research of direct relevance to education practice and policy. And yet neuroscience has had little impact on education. This might in part be due to a lack of interaction between educators and brain scientists. This in turn might be because of difficulties of translating the neuroscience knowledge of how learning takes place in the brain into information of value to teachers. It is here where we try to fill a gap. Interdisciplinary dialogue needs a mediator to prevent one or other discipline dominating, and, notwithstanding John Bruer’s remarks that it is cognitive psychology that ‘bridges the gap’ between neuroscience and education (Bruer, 1997), we feel that now is the time to explore the implications of brain science itself for education.

[1]  D. Hubel,et al.  Extent of recovery from the effects of visual deprivation in kittens. , 1965, Journal of neurophysiology.

[2]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. , 2000, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[3]  J B Poline,et al.  Partially overlapping neural networks for real and imagined hand movements. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[4]  W. Greenough,et al.  Experience and brain development. , 1987, Child development.

[5]  P. Huttenlocher Synaptic density in human frontal cortex - developmental changes and effects of aging. , 1979, Brain research.

[6]  Vicki A. Anderson,et al.  Development of Executive Functions Through Late Childhood and Adolescence in an Australian Sample , 2001, Developmental neuropsychology.

[7]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[8]  Usha Goswami The brain in the classroom? The state of the art. , 2005, Developmental science.

[9]  M. Hallett,et al.  Activation of the primary visual cortex by Braille reading in blind subjects , 1996, Nature.

[10]  Judy Reilly,et al.  Cognitive efficiency on a match to sample task decreases at the onset of puberty in children , 2002, Brain and Cognition.

[11]  P. Rakić Corticogenesis in human and nonhuman primates. , 1995 .

[12]  Paul Howard-Jones An invaluable foundation for better bridges. , 2005, Developmental science.

[13]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[14]  P. Yakovlev,et al.  The myelogenetic cycles of regional maturation of the brain , 1967 .

[15]  J. Decety,et al.  The effects of learning and intention on the neural network involved in the perception of meaningless actions. , 1999, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[16]  T J Sejnowski,et al.  Running enhances neurogenesis, learning, and long-term potentiation in mice. , 1999, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[17]  K. Nakayama,et al.  Sleep-dependent learning: a nap is as good as a night , 2003, Nature Neuroscience.

[18]  John T. Bruer,et al.  Education and the Brain: A Bridge Too Far , 1997 .

[19]  D. L. Stewart,et al.  Reversal of structural and functional effects of long-term visual deprivation in cats. , 1972, Experimental neurology.

[20]  F. Fazio,et al.  Dyslexia: Cultural Diversity and Biological Unity , 2001, Science.

[21]  Axel Cleeremans,et al.  Experience-dependent changes in cerebral activation during human REM sleep , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  J. Kleim,et al.  A brain adaptation view of plasticity: is synaptic plasticity an overly limited concept? , 2002, Progress in brain research.

[23]  Lauren Stewart,et al.  Brain changes after learning to read and play music , 2003, NeuroImage.

[24]  S. Kosslyn,et al.  Brain areas underlying visual mental imagery and visual perception: an fMRI study. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[25]  M. Rutter,et al.  Are there biological programming effects for psychological development? Findings from a study of Romanian adoptees. , 2004, Developmental psychology.

[26]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Arithmetic and the Brain This Review Comes from a Themed Issue on Cognitive Neuroscience Edited the Intraparietal Sulcus and Number Sense Number Sense in the Animal Brain , 2022 .

[27]  Daniel Ansari Paving the way towards meaningful interactions between neuroscience and education. , 2005, Developmental science.