If at First You Don't Succeed: The Role of Evidence in Preschoolers' and Infants' Persistence

Perseverance, above and beyond IQ, predicts academic outcomes in school age children. Yet, little is known about how very young children learn the contexts in which persistence is valuable (or not). Here, we explore how young children and infants learn about the rational deployment of effort through observing adults’ persistent behavior. Results from Experiments 1 and 2 indicate that preschoolers persist more after watching an adult persist, but only if the adult is successful at reaching their goal. Experiment 3 extends these findings, showing that even infants use adult models to modulate their persistence, and can generalize this inference to novel situations. Thus, both preschoolers and infants are sensitive to adult persistence and use it to calibrate their own tenacity.

[1]  Understanding the Intentions of Others : Developmental Milestones , 2012 .

[2]  L. Schulz,et al.  Sins of omission: Children selectively explore when teachers are under-informative , 2014, Cognition.

[3]  A. Meltzoff Understanding the Intentions of Others: Re-Enactment of Intended Acts by 18-Month-Old Children. , 1995, Developmental psychology.

[4]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  The grit effect: predicting retention in the military, the workplace, school and marriage , 2013, Front. Psychol..

[5]  E. Markman,et al.  Prior experiences and perceived efficacy influence 3-year-olds' imitation. , 2008, Developmental psychology.

[6]  Barry J. Zimmerman,et al.  Effects of model persistence and statements of confidence on children's self-efficacy and problem solving. , 1981 .

[7]  Frank C Keil,et al.  Early understanding of the division of cognitive labor. , 2002, Child development.

[8]  P. Harris Trusting What You’re Told , 2012 .

[9]  Richard L. Mercer,et al.  WHERE Ω , 1973, APLQ.

[10]  Josef Perner,et al.  Children's Understanding of Informational Access as Source of Knowledge. , 1988 .

[11]  L. Schulz,et al.  Serious fun: preschoolers engage in more exploratory play when evidence is confounded. , 2007, Developmental psychology.

[12]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Developmental psychology: Rational imitation in preverbal infants , 2002, Nature.

[13]  C. Dweck,et al.  Individual differences in achievement goals among young children. , 1994, Child development.

[14]  Adrianna C. Jenkins,et al.  Judicious imitation: children differentially imitate deterministically and probabilistically effective actions. , 2008, Child development.

[15]  Andrei Cimpian,et al.  The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation. , 2010, Developmental psychology.

[16]  C. Legare Exploring explanation: explaining inconsistent evidence informs exploratory, hypothesis-testing behavior in young children. , 2012, Child development.

[17]  Andrew G. Young,et al.  The hidden structure of overimitation , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[18]  Lisa S. Blackwell,et al.  Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: a longitudinal study and an intervention. , 2007, Child development.

[19]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  Where science starts: Spontaneous experiments in preschoolers’ exploratory play , 2011, Cognition.

[20]  C. Dweck,et al.  Mindsets That Promote Resilience: When Students Believe That Personal Characteristics Can Be Developed , 2012 .

[21]  A. Bandura Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. , 1977, Psychological review.

[22]  Angela L. Duckworth,et al.  Self-Discipline Outdoes IQ in Predicting Academic Performance of Adolescents , 2005, Psychological science.

[23]  Noah D. Goodman,et al.  The double-edged sword of pedagogy: Instruction limits spontaneous exploration and discovery , 2011, Cognition.

[24]  W. Schneider Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking , 1998 .

[25]  Fritz Holle,et al.  Effects of the Model , 1980 .

[26]  J. Tenenbaum,et al.  Not So Innocent , 2015, Psychological science.

[27]  G. Cumming Replication and p Intervals: p Values Predict the Future Only Vaguely, but Confidence Intervals Do Much Better , 2008, Perspectives on psychological science : a journal of the Association for Psychological Science.

[28]  H. Bekkering,et al.  Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. , 2000, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[29]  Susan Goldin-Meadow,et al.  Parent praise to 1- to 3-year-olds predicts children's motivational frameworks 5 years later. , 2013, Child development.

[30]  B. Kenward,et al.  Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learning , 2011, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.

[31]  B. Zimmerman,et al.  Effects of Model Persistence and Success on Children's Problem Solving. , 1979 .

[32]  Andrew Whiten,et al.  Imitation of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 3- and 5-year-old children , 2007 .

[33]  C. Dweck,et al.  Helplessness in early childhood: the role of contingent worth. , 1995, Child development.