A comparison of global motion perception using a multiple-aperture stimulus.

The human visual system integrates local motion signals to generate globally coherent motion percepts. However, it is unclear whether the perception of different types of global motion relies on a common motion integration mechanism. Using the multiple-aperture stimulus developed by K. Amano, M. Edwards, D. R. Badcock, and S. Nishida (2009), we compared the motion sensitivity (in terms of coherence threshold) for translational, circular, and radial motion. We found greater motion sensitivity for the two complex (circular and radial) motion types than for translational motion, implying that specific motion integration mechanisms are involved in the computation for different motion types. Our results reveal a "complexity advantage" in perceiving motion, which is consistent with physiological and computational evidence suggesting that specific mechanisms exist for processing complex circular/radial motion. We further examined the contributions of several critical factors that influence human global motion sensitivity. We found that human sensitivity for all motion types remained constant across a range of motion sampling density but varied depending on global speed. The minimum stimulus duration required for observers to reach constant sensitivity was found to be short ( approximately 140 ms) for all motion types.

[1]  Scott A. Beardsley,et al.  Psychophysical evidence for a radial motion bias in complex motion discrimination , 2005, Vision Research.

[2]  K. H. Britten,et al.  Responses of neurons in macaque MT to stochastic motion signals , 1993, Visual Neuroscience.

[3]  David R Badcock,et al.  Asymmetries in the Sensitivity to Motion in Depth: A Centripetal Bias , 1993, Perception.

[4]  H Barlow,et al.  Correspondence Noise and Signal Pooling in the Detection of Coherent Visual Motion , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  James T. Todd,et al.  The perception of globally coherent motion , 1992, Vision Research.

[6]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: Interaction of the ON and OFF pathways , 1994, Vision Research.

[7]  Hideo Sakata,et al.  Functional properties of rotation-sensitive neurons in the posterior parietal association cortex of the monkey , 1994, Experimental Brain Research.

[8]  Frank Bremmer,et al.  Neural correlates of implied motion , 2003, Nature.

[9]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[10]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  How MT cells analyze the motion of visual patterns , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[11]  C. Clifford,et al.  The perception and discrimination of speed in complex motion , 1999, Vision Research.

[12]  Randolph Blake,et al.  Detection and discrimination of optical flow components , 1998 .

[13]  D. Burr,et al.  Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.

[14]  Robert F. Hess,et al.  Second-order optic flow processing , 2007, Vision Research.

[15]  David C. Burr,et al.  Cardinal axes for radial and circular motion, revealed by summation and by masking , 2001, Vision Research.

[16]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[17]  D. Burr,et al.  Two stages of visual processing for radial and circular motion , 1995, Nature.

[18]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[19]  D. Badcock,et al.  Global-motion Perception: Interaction of Chromatic and Luminance Signals , 1996, Vision Research.

[20]  H. Wilson,et al.  Detection of global structure in Glass patterns: implications for form vision , 1998, Vision Research.

[21]  David R. Badcock,et al.  Coherent global motion in the absence of coherent velocity signals , 2000, Current Biology.

[22]  Jocelyn Faubert,et al.  How is complex second-order motion processed? , 2003, Vision Research.

[23]  Andrew T. Smith,et al.  The Representation of Egomotion in the Human Brain , 2008, Current Biology.

[24]  G. Orban,et al.  The Speed Tuning of Medial Superior Temporal (Mst) Cell Responses to Optic-Flow Components , 1995, Perception.

[25]  Edward H. Adelson,et al.  Motion illusions as optimal percepts , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.

[26]  S. Nishida,et al.  Adaptive pooling of visual motion signals by the human visual system revealed with a novel multi-element stimulus. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[27]  Norberto M. Grzywacz,et al.  A computational theory for the perception of coherent visual motion , 1988, Nature.

[28]  D. Badcock,et al.  Global motion perception: No interaction between the first- and second-order motion pathways , 1995, Vision Research.

[29]  Alan L. Yuille,et al.  Ideal Observers for Detecting Motion: Correspondence Noise , 2005, NIPS.

[30]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Medial Superior Temporal Area Neurons Respond to Speed Patterns in Optic Flow , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[31]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[32]  D R Badcock,et al.  Independent first- and second-order motion energy analyses of optic flow , 2001, Psychological research.

[33]  E. Adelson,et al.  Directionally selective complex cells and the computation of motion energy in cat visual cortex , 1992, Vision Research.

[34]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[35]  Hongjing Lu,et al.  Superior perception of circular/radial than translational motion cannot be explained by generic priors , 2010 .

[36]  T. Freeman,et al.  Human sensitivity to expanding and rotating motion: effects of complementary masking and directional structure , 1992, Vision Research.

[37]  U. Ahlström,et al.  Segregation of Motion Structure from Random Visual Noise , 1996, Perception.

[38]  A. Metha,et al.  Enhanced motion aftereffect for complex motions , 1999, Vision Research.

[39]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Underlying mechanisms of the response specificity of expansion/contraction and rotation cells in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  D. Burr,et al.  Temporal integration of optic flow, measured by contrast and coherence thresholds , 2001, Vision Research.

[41]  R. Wurtz,et al.  Sensitivity of MST neurons to optic flow stimuli. I. A continuum of response selectivity to large-field stimuli. , 1991, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.