Attention Protects the Fidelity of Visual Memory: Behavioral and Electrophysiological Evidence

Recall from visual memory is vulnerable to the influence of task-irrelevant information, including the remembered, prototypical value of stimuli seen previously. Wilken and Ma (2004) proposed that this distortion of recall was actually adaptive, with the task-irrelevant information compensating for imperfections of memory. We tested their proposal by using trial-by-trial oscillations in the electroencephalogram's alpha band (8–14 Hz) collected from human subjects as a marker for the strength of visual attention. Subjects' recall of stimulus spatial frequency showed a systematic error, namely a shift toward the prototypical value of previously seen stimuli. The magnitude of this prototype effect was strongly related to the amplitude of alpha band oscillations recorded at posterior sensor locations during the first 100 ms after stimulus onset. Our results support the hypothesis that the prototype effect is compensatory for imprecision in memory. Moreover, the attentional modulation of alpha activity during the encoding of the target stimulus is consistent with the view that attention sharpens the neural responses that are elicited by the task-relevant stimulus.

[1]  O. Jensen,et al.  Modulation of Gamma and Alpha Activity during a Working Memory Task Engaging the Dorsal or Ventral Stream , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[2]  G. Pfurtscheller Event-related synchronization (ERS): an electrophysiological correlate of cortical areas at rest. , 1992, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.

[3]  Yvonne Höller,et al.  Functional similarities between the P1 component and alpha oscillations , 2008, The European journal of neuroscience.

[4]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  α-Band Electroencephalographic Activity over Occipital Cortex Indexes Visuospatial Attention Bias and Predicts Visual Target Detection , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[5]  G. Mangun,et al.  The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  C. Schroeder,et al.  Neuronal Mechanisms of Cortical Alpha Oscillations in Awake-Behaving Macaques , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[7]  Arnaud Delorme,et al.  EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.

[8]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Optimal sustained attention is linked to the spectral content of background EEG activity: greater ongoing tonic alpha (approximately 10 Hz) power supports successful phasic goal activation. , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[9]  R. VanRullen,et al.  The Phase of Ongoing EEG Oscillations Predicts Visual Perception , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[10]  A. Watson,et al.  Quest: A Bayesian adaptive psychometric method , 1983, Perception & psychophysics.

[11]  Andreas Kleinschmidt,et al.  EEG-correlated fMRI of human alpha activity , 2003, NeuroImage.

[12]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[13]  W. Ma,et al.  A detection theory account of change detection. , 2004, Journal of vision.

[14]  Manuel Schabus,et al.  Phase-locked alpha and theta oscillations generate the P1-N1 complex and are related to memory performance. , 2004, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[15]  G. V. Simpson,et al.  Anticipatory Biasing of Visuospatial Attention Indexed by Retinotopically Specific α-Bank Electroencephalography Increases over Occipital Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  Á. Pascual-Leone,et al.  Spontaneous fluctuations in posterior alpha-band EEG activity reflect variability in excitability of human visual areas. , 2008, Cerebral cortex.

[17]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Distortions in recall from visual memory: two classes of attractors at work. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[18]  M S Landy,et al.  Ideal cue combination for localizing texture-defined edges. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[19]  T. Sejnowski,et al.  Dynamic Brain Sources of Visual Evoked Responses , 2002, Science.

[20]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Parieto‐occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load , 2007, Human brain mapping.

[21]  M. Carrasco,et al.  Attention alters appearance , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[22]  Wei Ji Ma,et al.  Spiking networks for Bayesian inference and choice , 2008, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[23]  F. L. D. Silva,et al.  Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.

[24]  G. Thut,et al.  Mechanisms of selective inhibition in visual spatial attention are indexed by α‐band EEG synchronization , 2007, The European journal of neuroscience.

[25]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Increases in alpha oscillatory power reflect an active retinotopic mechanism for distracter suppression during sustained visuospatial attention. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[26]  M. Carrasco,et al.  A population-coding model of attention’s influence on contrast response: Estimating neural effects from psychophysical data , 2009, Vision Research.

[27]  G. Pfurtscheller,et al.  Functional brain imaging based on ERD/ERS , 2001, Vision Research.

[28]  M. Ernst Learning to integrate arbitrary signals from vision and touch. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[29]  G. Mangun,et al.  Covariations in ERP and PET measures of spatial selective attention in human extrastriate visual cortex , 1997, Human brain mapping.

[30]  G. Humphreys,et al.  Visual marking: prioritizing selection for new objects by top-down attentional inhibition of old objects. , 1997, Psychological review.

[31]  J. P. Thomas,et al.  Why are some spatial discriminations independent of contrast? , 1989, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[32]  Konrad Paul Kording,et al.  Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning , 2004, Nature.

[33]  R. Knight,et al.  Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing , 2008, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[34]  Jerome R. Busemeyer,et al.  A New Method for Investigating Prototype Learning , 1988 .

[35]  Eero P. Simoncelli,et al.  Noise characteristics and prior expectations in human visual speed perception , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.

[36]  J. Reynolds,et al.  Attentional modulation of visual processing. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[37]  Hellmuth Obrig,et al.  Correlates of alpha rhythm in functional magnetic resonance imaging and near infrared spectroscopy , 2003, NeuroImage.

[38]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Neural Suppression of Irrelevant Information Underlies Optimal Working Memory Performance , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[39]  Fernando Henrique Lopes da Silva,et al.  The hemodynamic response of the alpha rhythm: An EEG/fMRI study , 2007, NeuroImage.

[40]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[41]  W. Klimesch EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis , 1999, Brain Research Reviews.

[42]  Wolfgang Klimesch,et al.  Dissociation between phase‐locked and nonphase‐locked alpha oscillations in a working memory task , 2009, Human brain mapping.

[43]  W. Klimesch,et al.  EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top‐down processing in a working memory task , 2005, Human brain mapping.

[44]  Denis G. Pelli,et al.  THE DESIGN OF A NEW LETTER CHART FOR MEASURING CONTRAST SENSITIVITY , 1988 .

[45]  J. Schoffelen,et al.  Prestimulus Oscillatory Activity in the Alpha Band Predicts Visual Discrimination Ability , 2008, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[46]  E. Vogel,et al.  Sensory gain control (amplification) as a mechanism of selective attention: electrophysiological and neuroimaging evidence. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[47]  W. Klimesch,et al.  Alpha phase synchronization predicts P1 and N1 latency and amplitude size. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[48]  John J. Foxe,et al.  Optimal sustained attention is linked to the spectral content of background EEG activity: greater ongoing tonic alpha (∼10 Hz) power supports successful phasic goal activation , 2007 .

[49]  Adam Gazzaley,et al.  Practice-related improvement in working memory is modulated by changes in processing external interference. , 2009, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  D. Knill Robust cue integration: a Bayesian model and evidence from cue-conflict studies with stereoscopic and figure cues to slant. , 2007, Journal of vision.

[51]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Increased Activity in Human Visual Cortex during Directed Attention in the Absence of Visual Stimulation , 1999, Neuron.

[52]  S. Treue,et al.  Feature-Based Attention Increases the Selectivity of Population Responses in Primate Visual Cortex , 2004, Current Biology.

[53]  M. Ernst,et al.  Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion , 2002, Nature.

[54]  N. P. Bichot,et al.  Spatial selection via feature-driven inhibition of distractor locations , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.

[55]  J. Lisman,et al.  Oscillations in the alpha band (9-12 Hz) increase with memory load during retention in a short-term memory task. , 2002, Cerebral cortex.

[56]  Daniel M Wolpert,et al.  Bayesian integration in force estimation. , 2004, Journal of neurophysiology.

[57]  S J Luck,et al.  Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays. II. Functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.