Connectionist Models in Cognitive Neuroscience

We review three problems in the connectionist modelling of visual word recognition: the restriction of models to monosyllabic words, the difficulty in assimilating fixation data from the reading of continuous text, and the abstractness of the accounts of dyslexic reading. We show how a model of visual word recognition, the SPLIT model, can be anatomically based on the precise splitting of the foveal projection about a vertical meridian. The SPLIT model has a limited instantiation as a connectionist model and a wider instantiation as a conventional statistical analysis. This combination of two modelling paradigms, both based on foveal splitting, gives the best coverage of word recognition phenomena.

[1]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Representation of Grammatical Categories of Words in the Brain , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[2]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Schemata and Sequential Thought Processes in PDP Models , 1986 .

[3]  Peter Földiák,et al.  SPARSE CODING IN THE PRIMATE CORTEX , 2002 .

[4]  F. Lhermitte 'Utilization behaviour' and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[5]  Stephen A. Ritz,et al.  Distinctive features, categorical perception, and probability learning: some applications of a neural model , 1977 .

[6]  PIERRE VAN DE LAAR,et al.  Task-Dependent Learning of Attention , 1997, Neural Networks.

[7]  Nick Chater,et al.  Connectionist modelling: Implications for cognitive neuropsychology , 1995 .

[8]  M Corbetta,et al.  Attentional modulation of neural processing of shape, color, and velocity in humans. , 1990, Science.

[9]  S. Tipper,et al.  Reaching into cluttered visual environments: spatial and temporal influences of distracting objects. , 1998, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[10]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Lesioning an attractor network: investigations of acquired dyslexia , 1991 .

[11]  E. Rosch,et al.  Family resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories , 1975, Cognitive Psychology.

[12]  C. Colby,et al.  The Neuroanatomy and Neurophysiology of Attention , 1991 .

[13]  I. Izquierdo,et al.  Memory Formation: The Sequence of Biochemical Events in the Hippocampus and Its Connection to Activity in Other Brain Structures , 1997, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.

[14]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  Competitive Learning: From Interactive Activation to Adaptive Resonance , 1987, Cogn. Sci..

[15]  D. Sparks,et al.  Population coding of saccadic eye movements by neurons in the superior colliculus , 1988, Nature.

[16]  John G. Taylor Temporal sequence storage , 1991 .

[17]  T. Shallice,et al.  Category specific semantic impairments , 1984 .

[18]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  On the nature and scope of featural representations of word meaning. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[19]  Susan Kemper,et al.  Grammatical abilities in Parkinson's disease: evidence from written sentences , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[20]  Steven W. Keele,et al.  Mechanisms of Attention. , 1978 .

[21]  E. Reed The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception , 1989 .

[22]  David E. Rumelhart,et al.  Brain style computation: learning and generalization , 1990 .

[23]  L. Squire Memory and the hippocampus: a synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. , 1992, Psychological review.

[24]  C. C. Wood Variations on a theme by Lashley: lesion experiments on the neural model of Anderson, Silverstein, Ritz, and Jones. , 1978, Psychological review.

[25]  R. H. Phaf,et al.  SLAM: A connectionist model for attention in visual selection tasks , 1990, Cognitive Psychology.

[26]  Jun Tanji,et al.  Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements ahead , 1994, Nature.

[27]  S. Judge,et al.  Neurons in the monkey midbrain with activity related to vergence eye movement and accommodation. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[28]  James L. McClelland,et al.  On the control of automatic processes: a parallel distributed processing account of the Stroop effect. , 1990, Psychological review.

[29]  Malti Patel Using Neural Nets to Investigate Lexical Access , 1996, PRICAI.

[30]  A. H. C. van der Heijden,et al.  Selective Attention in Vision , 1991 .

[31]  A. Georgopoulos Current issues in directional motor control , 1995, Trends in Neurosciences.

[32]  Carl P. Duncan,et al.  Retrieval of low-frequency words from mixed lists , 1974 .

[33]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention , 1987, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  S. File,et al.  Habituation and generalization of phobic responses to cat odor , 1994, Brain Research Bulletin.

[35]  B. Hood,et al.  The intention to act improves unilateral left neglect: two demonstrations , 1995, Neuroreport.

[36]  Glen A. Raser Recoding of semantic and acoustic information in short-term memory , 1972 .

[37]  E. Warrington,et al.  Categories of knowledge. Further fractionations and an attempted integration. , 1987, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[38]  Anna Maria Di Betta,et al.  The Production of Specific and Generic Associates of Living and Nonliving, High- and Low-Familiarity Stimuli in Alzheimer's Disease , 1996, Brain and Language.

[39]  U. Neisser Cognitive Psychology: Classic Edition , 1967 .

[40]  M. Humphreys,et al.  Different Ways to Cue a Coherent Memory System: A Theory for Episodic, Semantic, and Procedural Tasks. , 1989 .

[41]  A. Fuchs,et al.  Vertical eye movement-related responses of neurons in midbrain near intestinal nucleus of Cajal. , 1981, Journal of neurophysiology.

[42]  S. Tipper,et al.  Selective reaching: evidence for action-centered attention. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[43]  L. Abbott,et al.  Responses of neurons in primary and inferior temporal visual cortices to natural scenes , 1997, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.

[44]  Guido Gainotti,et al.  Cognitive and anatomical locus of lesion in a patient with a category-specific semantic impairment for living beings , 1996 .

[45]  G. E. Alexander,et al.  Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. , 1986, Annual review of neuroscience.

[46]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[47]  S. Luck,et al.  Bridging the Gap between Monkey Neurophysiology and Human Perception: An Ambiguity Resolution Theory of Visual Selective Attention , 1997, Cognitive Psychology.

[48]  John A. Bullinaria,et al.  Modelling Lexical Decision Using Corpus Derived Semantic Representations in a Connectionist Network , 1997, NCPW.

[49]  D Henson Oculomotor adaptation to induced deviations , 1982 .

[50]  Eric Saund,et al.  A Multiple Cause Mixture Model for Unsupervised Learning , 1995, Neural Computation.

[51]  E. Tulving,et al.  Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: positron emission tomography data. , 1994, Neuroreport.

[52]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Lexical organization of nouns and verbs in the brain , 1991, Nature.

[53]  Paul W. B. Atkins,et al.  Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. , 1993 .

[54]  R. Desimone,et al.  The representation of stimulus familiarity in anterior inferior temporal cortex. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[55]  D. Amaral,et al.  The entorhinal cortex of the monkey: II. Cortical afferents , 1987, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[56]  A P Georgopoulos Neural coding of the direction of reaching and a comparison with saccadic eye movements. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[57]  M. Tsodyks,et al.  The Enhanced Storage Capacity in Neural Networks with Low Activity Level , 1988 .

[58]  L. Jakobson,et al.  A neurological dissociation between perceiving objects and grasping them , 1991, Nature.

[59]  L. Manning Optic aphasia with spared action naming: a description and possible loci of impairment , 1997 .

[60]  G. Orban,et al.  Selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons for partially occluded shapes , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[61]  P. D. Mccormack,et al.  Recognition memory for common and rare words. , 1972, Journal of experimental psychology.

[62]  M. Gluck,et al.  Integrating behavioral and physiological models of hippocampal function , 1996, Hippocampus.

[63]  V. Gregg Word frequency, recognition and recall. , 1976 .

[64]  Murray Grossman,et al.  The Semantic Memory Impairment of Alzheimer's Disease: Category-Specific? , 1996, Cortex.

[65]  G W Humphreys,et al.  Visual search for targets defined by combinations of color, shape, and size: An examination of the task constraints on feature and conjunction searches , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.

[66]  Roderick I. Nicolson,et al.  Experimental and Connectionist Perspectives on Semantic Memory Development , 1998 .

[67]  Heekuck Oh,et al.  Neural Networks for Pattern Recognition , 1993, Adv. Comput..

[68]  George A. Miller,et al.  Introduction to WordNet: An On-line Lexical Database , 1990 .

[69]  Geoffrey E. Hinton Connectionist Learning Procedures , 1989, Artif. Intell..

[70]  P. Carpenter,et al.  Individual differences in working memory and reading , 1980 .

[71]  James S. Albus,et al.  Outline for a theory of intelligence , 1991, IEEE Trans. Syst. Man Cybern..

[72]  James L. McClelland,et al.  Why there are complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of connectionist models of learning and memory. , 1995, Psychological review.

[73]  J. Hodges,et al.  Semantic dementia. Progressive fluent aphasia with temporal lobe atrophy. , 1992 .

[74]  Richard J. Brown Neuropsychology Mental Structure , 1989 .

[75]  R. Passingham Two cortical systems for directing movement. , 1987, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[76]  D. Amaral,et al.  The three-dimensional organization of the hippocampal formation: A review of anatomical data , 1989, Neuroscience.

[77]  Joseph P. Levy,et al.  Interactions between short- and long-term weights: Applications for cognitive modelling. , 1995 .

[78]  L. R. Allen,et al.  The influence of word-knowledge on the word-frequency effect in recognition memory , 1968 .

[79]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  A model for recognition memory: REM—retrieving effectively from memory , 1997, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[80]  A. Baddeley,et al.  Recall of Semantic Clusters in Primary Memory , 1971 .

[81]  P. Cavanagh,et al.  Focused attention distorts visual space: an attentional repulsion effect. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[82]  S. Tipper,et al.  A model of inhibitory mechanisms in selective attention. , 1994 .

[83]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Perception of multiple objects - a connectionist approach , 1991, Neural network modeling and connectionism.

[84]  D. C. Lee The Map-Building and Exploration Strategies of A Simple Sonar-Equipped Robot: Bibliography , 1996 .

[85]  Peter Ford Dominey,et al.  A Model of Corticostriatal Plasticity for Learning Oculomotor Associations and Sequences , 1995, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[86]  Douglas L. Hintzman,et al.  Recognition and recall in MINERVA 2: Analysis of the 'recognition-failure' paradigm. , 1987 .

[87]  R. Ratcliff,et al.  A retrieval theory of priming in memory. , 1988, Psychological review.

[88]  V. Meininger,et al.  Bilateral tactile aphasia: a tacto-verbal dysfunction. , 1978, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[89]  Michael Recce,et al.  A model of hippocampal 'place cells' for navigation , 1994 .

[90]  R. Desimone,et al.  Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. , 1985, Science.

[91]  J J Hopfield,et al.  Neurons with graded response have collective computational properties like those of two-state neurons. , 1984, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[92]  R. D. Patterson,et al.  Functional units and retrieval processes in free recall. , 1968, Journal of experimental psychology.

[93]  John A. Bullinaria,et al.  Modelling Lexical Decision: Who needs a lexicon? , 1995 .

[94]  A. Wilkins Conjoint frequency, category size, and categorization time , 1971 .

[95]  M. Patel,et al.  Modeling lexical-semantic processes using wordnet , 1998 .

[96]  W. Newsome,et al.  The Variable Discharge of Cortical Neurons: Implications for Connectivity, Computation, and Information Coding , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[97]  M I Posner,et al.  Anatomy of word and sentence meaning. , 1998, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[98]  Carlo Semenza,et al.  Auditory Modality-Specific Anomia: Evidence from a Case of Pure Word Deafness , 1975, Cortex.

[99]  K Tanaka,et al.  Neuronal mechanisms of object recognition. , 1993, Science.

[100]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[101]  Will Lowe Semantic Representation and Priming in a Self-organizing Lexicon , 1997, NCPW.

[102]  J. Metcalfe,et al.  Recognition failure and the composite memory trace in CHARM. , 1991, Psychological review.

[103]  C. D. Green,et al.  Are Connectionist Models Theories of Cognition , 1998 .

[104]  E. W. Kairiss,et al.  Hebbian synapses: biophysical mechanisms and algorithms. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[105]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Semantic and Associative Priming in a Distributed Attractor Network , 1995 .

[106]  G. Deco,et al.  An Information-Theoretic Approach to Neural Computing , 1997, Perspectives in Neural Computing.

[107]  M Glanzer,et al.  The mirror effect in recognition memory , 1984, Memory & cognition.

[108]  E T Rolls,et al.  Sparseness of the neuronal representation of stimuli in the primate temporal visual cortex. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[109]  R. Berndt,et al.  Category-specific naming deficit following cerebral infarction , 1985, Nature.

[110]  John G. Taylor,et al.  Models of Coupled Anterior Working Memories for Frontal Tasks , 1997, NCPW.

[111]  H B Barlow,et al.  Single units and sensation: a neuron doctrine for perceptual psychology? , 1972, Perception.

[112]  Richard DeWitt Vagueness, Semantics, and the Language of Thought , 1993 .

[113]  N. Schmajuk,et al.  Latent inhibition: a neural network approach. , 1996, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.

[114]  I. Robertson,et al.  One hand is better than two: Motor extinction of left hand advantage in unilateral neglect , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[115]  M. Goodale,et al.  The visual brain in action , 1995 .

[116]  James L. McClelland,et al.  A computational model of semantic memory impairment: modality specificity and emergent category specificity. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[117]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Mechanisms of Spatial Attention: The Relation of Macrostructure to Microstructure in Parietal Neglect , 1994, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[118]  T. Shallice,et al.  Perseverative and Semantic Influences on Visual Object Naming Errors in Optic Aphasia: A Connectionist Account , 1993, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[119]  Ferdinando A. Mussa-Ivaldi,et al.  Toward a neurobiology of coordinate transformations , 1995 .

[120]  R. Shiffrin,et al.  A retrieval model for both recognition and recall. , 1984, Psychological review.

[121]  R. Palmer,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[122]  R. O’Reilly,et al.  A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges. , 1996, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[123]  Jeffrey W. McCandless,et al.  A neural net model of the adaptation of binocular vertical eye alignment , 1997 .

[124]  H. Egeth,et al.  Searching for conjunctively defined targets. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[125]  John G. Taylor,et al.  Storing temporal sequences , 1991, Neural Networks.

[126]  A. Treisman,et al.  A feature-integration theory of attention , 1980, Cognitive Psychology.

[127]  R Vogels,et al.  Coding of stimulus invariances by inferior temporal neurons. , 1996, Progress in brain research.

[128]  John Hart,et al.  Neural subsystems for object knowledge , 1992, Nature.

[129]  T. Shallice Impairments of semantic processing: multiple dissociations , 1987 .

[130]  M. J. Riddoch,et al.  Visual object processing in optic aphasia: a case of semantic access agnosia , 1987 .

[131]  Trevor A. Harley,et al.  Phonological activation of semantic competitors during lexical access in speech production , 1993 .

[132]  A. Treisman,et al.  Conjunction search revisited. , 1990, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[133]  N. Cowan Activation, attention, and short-term memory , 1993, Memory & cognition.

[134]  S. Tipper,et al.  Inhibition and Interference in Selective Attention: Some Tests of a Neural Network Model , 1996 .

[135]  C. H. Vanderwolf,et al.  Hippocampal electrical activity during waking behaviour and sleep: analyses using centrally acting drugs. , 1977, Ciba Foundation symposium.

[136]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Double Dissociation of Semantic Categories in Alzheimer's Disease , 1997, Brain and Language.

[137]  L. Goldstein The frontal lobes and voluntary action , 1996 .

[138]  J O'Keefe,et al.  Robotic and neuronal simulation of the hippocampus and rat navigation. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[139]  E. Warrington Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory the Selective Impairment of Semantic Memory , 2022 .

[140]  B Behring,et al.  Progressive dementia with parkinsonism in corticobasal and brainstem degeneration with neuronal inclusions , 1998, Neurology.

[141]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  Laminar selectivity of the cholinergic suppression of synaptic transmission in rat hippocampal region CA1: computational modeling and brain slice physiology , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[142]  W. Beatty,et al.  Lexical Processing in Parkinson's Disease and Multiple Sclerosis , 1989, Journal of geriatric psychiatry and neurology.

[143]  T. Shallice,et al.  Deep Dyslexia: A Case Study of , 1993 .

[144]  S. Clark,et al.  Word frequency effects in associative and item recognition , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[145]  E. Rolls,et al.  Neural networks and brain function , 1998 .

[146]  Erminio Capitani,et al.  Perceptual and Associative Knowledge in Category Specific Impairment of Semantic Memory: A Study of two Cases , 1993, Cortex.

[147]  Malti Patel,et al.  Extracting Semantic Representations from Large Text Corpora , 1997, NCPW.

[148]  Sverker Sikström,et al.  The TECO Connectionist Theory of Recognition Failure , 1996 .

[149]  D. Ingle Selective choice between double prey objects by frogs. , 1973, Brain, behavior and evolution.

[150]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-organization and associative memory: 3rd edition , 1989 .

[151]  Richard D. Hichwa,et al.  A neural basis for lexical retrieval , 1996, Nature.

[152]  J. Duncan,et al.  Visual search and stimulus similarity. , 1989, Psychological review.

[153]  S P Tipper,et al.  Action-based mechanisms of attention. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[154]  Clifton M. Schor,et al.  Context-specific adaptation of vertical vergence to correlates of eye position , 1997, Vision Research.

[155]  Robert M. French,et al.  Could category-specific semantic deficits reflect differences in the distribution of features within a unified semantic memory? , 1998 .

[156]  S. Kornblum,et al.  Stimulus-response compatibility with relevant and irrelevant stimulus dimensions that do and do not overlap with the response. , 1995, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[157]  J. H. Neely Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. , 1991 .

[158]  B. Motter Neural correlates of attentive selection for color or luminance in extrastriate area V4 , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[159]  S. Nelson,et al.  An emergent model of orientation selectivity in cat visual cortical simple cells , 1995, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[160]  Clifton M. Schor,et al.  Mechanisms of vertical phoria adaptation revealed by time-course and two-dimensional spatiotopic maps , 1994, Vision Research.

[161]  Michael Frotscher,et al.  Cholinergic innervation of the rat hippocampus as revealed by choline acetyltransferase immunocytochemistry: A combined light and electron microscopic study , 1985, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[162]  V. Nechaev,et al.  Correlates of lexical processing in the activity of neuronal populations of the human brain. , 1990, Stereotactic and functional neurosurgery.

[163]  Wayne D. Gray,et al.  Basic objects in natural categories , 1976, Cognitive Psychology.

[164]  J. Gorell,et al.  Lexical decision in Parkinson disease: lack of evidence for generalized bradyphrenia. , 1994, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[165]  G. Orban,et al.  Cue-invariant shape selectivity of macaque inferior temporal neurons. , 1993, Science.

[166]  J H Maunsell,et al.  The Brain's Visual World: Representation of Visual Targets in Cerebral Cortex , 1995, Science.

[167]  J. Hopfield Neurons withgraded response havecollective computational properties likethoseoftwo-state neurons , 1984 .

[168]  A. Gorman Recognition memory for nouns as a function of abstractness and frequency. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[169]  Thomas E. Nichols,et al.  Anterior cingulate gyrus dysfunction and selective attention deficits in schizophrenia: [15O]H2O PET study during single-trial Stroop task performance. , 1997, The American journal of psychiatry.

[170]  B. C. Motter Focal attention produces spatially selective processing in visual cortical areas V1, V2, and V4 in the presence of competing stimuli. , 1993, Journal of neurophysiology.

[171]  A. Etienne The Control of Short-Distance Homing in the Golden Hamster , 1987 .

[172]  A. Caramazza,et al.  Domain-Specific Knowledge Systems in the Brain: The Animate-Inanimate Distinction , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[173]  Y Agid,et al.  Is the mesocortical dopaminergic system involved in Parkinson disease? , 1980, Neurology.

[174]  Dietmar Heinke,et al.  SAIM: A Model of Visual Attention and Neglect , 1997, ICANN.

[175]  Teuvo Kohonen,et al.  Self-Organization and Associative Memory , 1988 .

[176]  F. Lhermitte,et al.  A visual-speech disconnexion syndrome. Report of a case with optic aphasia, agnosic alexia and colour agnosia. , 1973, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[177]  Stephen Grossberg,et al.  A Theory of Human Memory: Self-Organization and Performance of Sensory-Motor Codes, Maps, and Plans , 1982 .

[178]  A. Damasio,et al.  A neural basis for the retrieval of conceptual knowledge , 1997, Neuropsychologia.

[179]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[180]  D. B. Bender,et al.  Visual properties of neurons in inferotemporal cortex of the Macaque. , 1972, Journal of neurophysiology.

[181]  David A. Balota,et al.  Test-Expectancy and Word-Frequency Effects in Recall and Recognition , 1980 .

[182]  Bernard Widrow,et al.  Adaptive switching circuits , 1988 .

[183]  S. C. Stanford,et al.  The Anxiogenic Agents, Yohimbine and FG 7142, Disrupt the Noradrenergic Response to Novelty , 1998, Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior.

[184]  Susan L. Franzel,et al.  Guided search: an alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[185]  Glyn W. Humphreys Dietmar Heinke,et al.  Spatial Representation and Selection in the Brain: Neuropsychological and Computational Constraints , 1998 .

[186]  E. Niebur,et al.  Modeling the Temporal Dynamics of IT Neurons in Visual Search: A Mechanism for Top-Down Selective Attention , 1996, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[187]  N. Kanwisher Repetition blindness: Type recognition without token individuation , 1987, Cognition.

[188]  M. Zorzi,et al.  A computational model of the Simon effect , 1995, Psychological research.

[189]  Martha J. Farah,et al.  Category-specificity and modality-specificity in semantic memory , 1989, Neuropsychologia.

[190]  J. Tanji,et al.  Neuronal activity in the primate supplementary, pre-supplementary and premotor cortex during externally and internally instructed sequential movements , 1994, Neuroscience Research.

[191]  J. Gurd,et al.  Word search in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1996, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[192]  E. Warrington,et al.  Category specific access dysphasia. , 2002, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[193]  B. Murdock,et al.  The retention of individual items. , 1961, Journal of experimental psychology.

[194]  J. Gurd,et al.  Frontal dissociations: evidence from Parkinson's disease , 1995, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[195]  M. Posner,et al.  The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.

[196]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Visual Working Memory in Prefrontal Cortex of the Macaque , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[197]  J. Duncan,et al.  Competitive brain activity in visual attention , 1997, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[198]  P. Dixon,et al.  University of Alberta norms of relative meaning frequency for 566 homographs , 1994, Memory & cognition.

[199]  Barak A. Pearlmutter Learning State Space Trajectories in Recurrent Neural Networks , 1989, Neural Computation.

[200]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Object vision and spatial vision: two cortical pathways , 1983, Trends in Neurosciences.

[201]  Allan Collins,et al.  A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing , 1975 .

[202]  J. Duncan,et al.  Beyond the search surface: visual search and attentional engagement. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[203]  Alfonso Caramazza,et al.  The Modality-Specific Organization of Grammatical Categories: Evidence from Impaired Spoken and Written Sentence Production , 1997, Brain and Language.

[204]  Lance J. Rips,et al.  Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations , 1973 .

[205]  H. J. Muller,et al.  SEarch via Recursive Rejection (SERR): A Connectionist Model of Visual Search , 1993, Cognitive Psychology.

[206]  Murray Grossman,et al.  Investigating the basis for the sentence comprehension deficit in Parkinson's disease , 1994, Journal of Neurolinguistics.

[207]  D. Plaut Double dissociation without modularity: evidence from connectionist neuropsychology. , 1995, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[208]  Juha Karhunen,et al.  Representation and separation of signals using nonlinear PCA type learning , 1994, Neural Networks.

[209]  R. Desimone Visual attention mediated by biased competition in extrastriate visual cortex. , 1998, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[210]  David C. Plaut,et al.  Systematicity and Specialization in Semantics: A Computational Account of Optic Aphasia , 1996 .

[211]  J. Illes,et al.  Language production in Parkinson's disease: Acoustic and linguistic considerations , 1988, Brain and Language.

[212]  C. Eriksen,et al.  Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task , 1974 .

[213]  Nora S. Newcombe,et al.  Semantic effects on ordered recall , 1976 .

[214]  A. Cools,et al.  Memory performance under varying cueing conditions in patients with Parkinson's disease , 1996, Neuropsychologia.

[215]  M. Humphreys,et al.  An auto-associative neural network for sparse representations : analysis and application to models of recognition and cued recall , 1994 .

[216]  D. Field,et al.  Natural image statistics and efficient coding. , 1996, Network.

[217]  M. Beauvois,et al.  Optic aphasia: a process of interaction between vision and language. , 1982, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[218]  Peter M. Todd,et al.  Learning and connectionist representations , 1993 .

[219]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Guided Search 2.0 A revised model of visual search , 1994, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[220]  B. Hommel,et al.  Blindness to response-compatible stimuli. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[221]  T. Powell,et al.  An anatomical study of converging sensory pathways within the cerebral cortex of the monkey. , 1970, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[222]  Saul Sternberg,et al.  The discovery of processing stages: Extensions of Donders' method , 1969 .

[223]  D. V. van Essen,et al.  A neurobiological model of visual attention and invariant pattern recognition based on dynamic routing of information , 1993, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[224]  Christopher M. Bishop,et al.  GTM: The Generative Topographic Mapping , 1998, Neural Computation.

[225]  K. Yorkston,et al.  Speech and pause characteristics following speech rate reduction in hypokinetic dysarthria. , 1996, Journal of communication disorders.

[226]  Michael C. Mozer,et al.  Computational modeling of spatial attention , 1996 .

[227]  Anders Krogh,et al.  Introduction to the theory of neural computation , 1994, The advanced book program.

[228]  L. Cipolotti,et al.  Dynamic aphasia: an inability to select between competing verbal responses? , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[229]  Mark E Nelson,et al.  Brain maps and parallel computers , 1990, Trends in Neurosciences.

[230]  Arthur P. Shimamura,et al.  Memory and frontal lobe function. , 1995 .

[231]  R. C. Honey,et al.  Hippocampal Lesions Disrupt an Associative Mismatch Process , 1998, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[232]  H. Coslett,et al.  Preserved object recognition and reading comprehension in optic aphasia. , 1989, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[233]  Joel L. Davis,et al.  Sensorimotor Selection and the Basal Ganglia: A Neural Network Model , 1994 .

[234]  H. Deubel,et al.  Selective Dorsal and Ventral Processing: Evidence for a Common Attentional Mechanism in Reaching and Perception , 1998 .

[235]  A. W. Melton Implications of short-term memory for a general theory of memory , 1963 .

[236]  Minami Ito,et al.  Columns for visual features of objects in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 1992, Nature.

[237]  J. Cohen,et al.  Context, cortex, and dopamine: a connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. , 1992, Psychological review.

[238]  W. H. Sumby,et al.  Word frequency and serial position effects , 1963 .

[239]  Arthur I. Schulman,et al.  Recognition memory for words presented at a slow or rapid rate , 1970 .

[240]  E K Warrington,et al.  Concrete word dyslexia. , 1981, British journal of psychology.

[241]  James L. McClelland,et al.  An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of basic findings. , 1981 .

[242]  S. Tipper The Negative Priming Effect: Inhibitory Priming by Ignored Objects , 1985, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[243]  M. Turk,et al.  Eigenfaces for Recognition , 1991, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[244]  H. Spitzer,et al.  Temporal encoding of two-dimensional patterns by single units in primate inferior temporal cortex. I. Response characteristics. , 1987, Journal of neurophysiology.

[245]  R. Atkinson,et al.  The control of short-term memory. , 1971, Scientific American.

[246]  Michael I. Jordan,et al.  Computational Consequences of a Bias toward Short Connections , 1992, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[247]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task , 1997, Nature.

[248]  J. Gorell,et al.  Impaired set-shifting in Parkinson's disease: new evidence from a lexical decision task. , 1996, Journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology.

[249]  M. Zorzi,et al.  Two routes or one in reading aloud? A connectionist dual-process model. , 1998 .

[250]  Rosaleen A. McCarthy,et al.  Multiple meaning systems in the brain: A case for visual semantics , 1994, Neuropsychologia.

[251]  R. Desimone,et al.  Attentional control of visual perception: cortical and subcortical mechanisms. , 1990, Cold Spring Harbor symposia on quantitative biology.

[252]  S. Amari,et al.  Competition and Cooperation in Neural Nets , 1982 .

[253]  Geoffrey E. Hinton,et al.  Hierarchical Non-linear Factor Analysis and Topographic Maps , 1997, NIPS.

[254]  S. Judge,et al.  How is binocularity maintained during convergence and divergence? , 1996, Eye.

[255]  M. Posner,et al.  Attention and the detection of signals. , 1980, Journal of experimental psychology.

[256]  Steven L. Small,et al.  Distributed representations of semantic knowledge in the brain. , 1995, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[257]  M. Ross Quillian,et al.  Retrieval time from semantic memory , 1969 .

[258]  A. Lees Cognitive Deficits in Parkinson’s Disease , 1985 .

[259]  John Duncan,et al.  A neural basis for visual search in inferior temporal cortex , 1993, Nature.

[260]  Adrienne Lehrer,et al.  Frames, Fields, and Contrasts : New Essays in Semantic and Lexical Organization , 1992 .

[261]  J. Eich A composite holographic associative recall model. , 1982 .

[262]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Category-Specific Semantic Deficits in Focal and Widespread Brain Damage: A Computational Account , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[263]  Richard M. Shiffrin,et al.  Cuing effects and associative information in recognition memory , 1992, Memory & cognition.

[264]  Mark S. Seidenberg,et al.  Priming and semantic memory loss in Alzheimer's disease , 1989, Brain and Language.

[265]  E. Rolls A theory of hippocampal function in memory , 1996, Hippocampus.

[266]  E. Tulving,et al.  Novelty assessment in the brain and long-term memory encoding , 1995, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[267]  J J Hopfield,et al.  Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. , 1982, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[268]  Edward E. Smith,et al.  Categories and concepts , 1984 .

[269]  J. Gurd,et al.  Competitive Inhibition Models of Lexical–Semantic Processing: Experimental Evidence , 1996, Brain and Language.