Inflexible neurobiological signatures precede atypical development in infants at high risk for autism

Variability in neurobiological signatures is ubiquitous in early life but the link to adverse developmental milestones in humans is unknown. We examined how levels of signal and noise in movement signatures during the 1st year of life constrain early development in 71 healthy typically developing infants, either at High or Low familial Risk (HR or LR, respectively) for developing Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD). Delays in early learning developmental trajectories in HR infants (validated in an analysis of 1,445 infants from representative infant-sibling studies) were predicted by worse stochastic patterns in their spontaneous head movements as early as 1–2 months after birth, relative to HR infants who showed more rapid developmental progress, as well as relative to all LR infants. While LR 1–2 mo-old infants’ movements were significantly different during a language listening task compared to during sleep, HR infants’ movements were more similar during both conditions, a striking lack of diversity that reveals context-inflexible experience of ambient information. Contrary to expectation, it is not the level of variability per se that is particularly detrimental in early life. Rather, inflexible sensorimotor systems and/or atypical transition between behavioral states may interfere with the establishment of capacity to extract structure and important cues from sensory input at birth, preceding and contributing to an atypical brain developmental trajectory in toddlerhood.

[1]  D. Teller The forced-choice preferential looking procedure: A psychophysical technique for use with human infants. , 1979 .

[2]  M. Goldsmith,et al.  Statistical Learning by 8-Month-Old Infants , 1996 .

[3]  B. Peterson,et al.  Cortical interactions during the resolution of information processing demands in autism spectrum disorders , 2016, Brain and behavior.

[4]  M. Blumberg,et al.  Self-Generated Movements with “Unexpected” Sensory Consequences , 2014, Current Biology.

[5]  H. Prechtl,et al.  The behavioural states of the newborn infant (a review). , 1974, Brain research.

[6]  S. Dehaene,et al.  Functional Neuroimaging of Speech Perception in Infants , 2002, Science.

[7]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric mapping , 2013 .

[8]  B. McMurray,et al.  Development of Twitching in Sleeping Infant Mice Depends on Sensory Experience , 2015 .

[9]  Gentaro Taga,et al.  Specific characteristics of spontaneous movements in preterm infants at term age are associated with developmental delays at age 3 years , 2013, Developmental medicine and child neurology.

[10]  Anne H. O'Donnell,et al.  Age-related sperm DNA methylation changes are transmitted to offspring and associated with abnormal behavior and dysregulated gene expression , 2014, Molecular Psychiatry.

[11]  J. Brian,et al.  Non‐ASD outcomes at 36 months in siblings at familial risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD): A baby siblings research consortium (BSRC) study , 2016, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[12]  A. Richdale,et al.  Sleep problems in autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, nature, & possible biopsychosocial aetiologies. , 2009, Sleep medicine reviews.

[13]  P. Jusczyk,et al.  Infants′ Sensitivity to the Sound Patterns of Native Language Words , 1993 .

[14]  Abraham Z. Snyder,et al.  Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion , 2012, NeuroImage.

[15]  Peter W. Jusczyk,et al.  How infants begin to extract words from speech , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[16]  H. Zoghbi Postnatal Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Meeting at the Synapse? , 2003, Science.

[17]  E. Newport,et al.  Science Current Directions in Psychological Statistical Learning : from Acquiring Specific Items to Forming General Rules on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[18]  Caroline F. Rowland,et al.  Visual sequence learning and language development: Evidence for a domain-general learning mechanism? , 2019 .

[19]  K. Denisova,et al.  Motor noise is rich signal in autism research and pharmacological treatments , 2016, Scientific Reports.

[20]  Jacob Feldman,et al.  Investigating shape representation using sensitivity to part- and axis-based transformations , 2016, Vision Research.

[21]  Mark H. Johnson,et al.  Mid‐childhood outcomes of infant siblings at familial high‐risk of autism spectrum disorder , 2016, Autism research : official journal of the International Society for Autism Research.

[22]  P. Kuhl,et al.  Infant speech perception activates Broca's area: a developmental magnetoencephalography study , 2006, Neuroreport.

[23]  S. Bryson,et al.  Recurrence Risk for Autism Spectrum Disorders: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium Study , 2011, Pediatrics.

[24]  Jennifer C. Sarrett,et al.  Commentary: Attention to Eyes Is Present but in Decline in 2–6-Month-Old Infants Later Diagnosed with Autism , 2015, Front. Public Health.

[25]  Friedemann Pulvermüller,et al.  Motor cortex maps articulatory features of speech sounds , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[26]  U. Fano Ionization Yield of Radiations. II. The Fluctuations of the Number of Ions , 1947 .

[27]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  Compression in visual working memory: using statistical regularities to form more efficient memory representations. , 2009, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[28]  S. Bryson,et al.  Early sex differences are not autism-specific: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium (BSRC) study , 2015, Molecular Autism.

[29]  J. Brian,et al.  Developmental Trajectories of Infants With Multiplex Family Risk for Autism: A Baby Siblings Research Consortium Study. , 2019, JAMA neurology.

[30]  E. Lenneberg On explaining language. , 1969, Science.

[31]  Guido Gerig,et al.  The Emergence of Network Inefficiencies in Infants With Autism Spectrum Disorder , 2017, Biological Psychiatry.

[32]  Timothy F. Brady,et al.  PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Statistical Learning Using Real-World Scenes Extracting Categorical Regularities Without Conscious Intent , 2022 .

[33]  G. Dehaene-Lambertz,et al.  The Infancy of the Human Brain , 2015, Neuron.

[34]  Charles Kemp,et al.  How to Grow a Mind: Statistics, Structure, and Abstraction , 2011, Science.

[35]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  Infant vocalizations in response to speech: vocal imitation and developmental change. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[36]  Toshiaki Imada,et al.  Infants’ brain responses to speech suggest Analysis by Synthesis , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[37]  M. Dylan Tisdall,et al.  Head motion during MRI acquisition reduces gray matter volume and thickness estimates , 2015, NeuroImage.

[38]  Melissa M. Kibbe,et al.  Infants use temporal regularities to chunk objects in memory , 2016, Cognition.

[39]  LouAnn Gerken,et al.  Decisions, decisions: infant language learning when multiple generalizations are possible , 2006, Cognition.

[40]  Scott P. Johnson,et al.  Visual statistical learning in infancy: evidence for a domain general learning mechanism , 2002, Cognition.

[41]  F. Guenther,et al.  The perceptual magnet effect as an emergent property of neural map formation. , 1996, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Spatial registration and normalization of images , 1995 .

[43]  A. Kondrashov,et al.  Genetics: The rate of human mutation , 2012, Nature.

[44]  L. Schulz,et al.  16-Month-Olds Rationally Infer Causes of Failed Actions , 2011, Science.

[45]  Kristine H. Onishi,et al.  Infants learn phonotactic regularities from brief auditory experience , 2003, Cognition.

[46]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Movement‐Related effects in fMRI time‐series , 1996, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[47]  Michael Wigler,et al.  The role of de novo mutations in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders , 2014, Nature Reviews Genetics.