Observational learning without a model is influenced by the observer’s possibility to act: Evidence from the Simon task

We assessed whether observational learning in perceptual-motor tasks is affected by the visibility of an action producing perceived environmental effects and by the observer's possibility to act during observation. To this end, we conducted three experiments in which participants were required to observe a spatial compatibility task in which only the effects of computer-generated responses were visible before executing a Simon task. In Experiment 1, we compared the effects of a passively observed practice with either a spatially compatible or incompatible stimulus-response (S-R) association. In Experiment 2, during the observed spatially incompatible practice participants were prevented from potentially acting, either because a plexiglas barrier separated the participant from the response device rendering it out of reach; or because the participant's hands were tied; or the device affording a response was absent. In Experiment 3, the plexiglas presented an opening that could allow the participant to potentially reach and interact with it. As when the practice is physically performed, we found an elimination of the Simon effect following a spatially incompatible observed practice, suggesting that participants learned an incompatible S-R association by observing and transferred this knowledge to the subsequent Simon task. No evidence of transfer of learning was found when, during passive observation, the participant's hands were tied, or a barrier prevented him/her from potentially interacting with the device, or no response device was present. Differently, a transfer-of-learning effect was observed when the barrier presented an opening. These results suggest that learning can derive from the mere observation of action effects, even when an action is not visible, as long as the observer has the potential to act.

[1]  B. Hommel,et al.  Effect anticipation and action control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[2]  J. R. Simon,et al.  Auditory S-R compatibility: the effect of an irrelevant cue on information processing. , 1967, The Journal of applied psychology.

[3]  V. Gallese,et al.  How the body in action shapes the self , 2011 .

[4]  R. Passingham,et al.  Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.

[5]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Motor activation prior to observation of a predicted movement , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[6]  F. Doricchi,et al.  Determining priority between attentional and referential-coding sources of the Simon effect through optokinetic stimulation , 2010, Neuropsychologia.

[7]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action recognition in the premotor cortex. , 1996, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[8]  C. Umilta,et al.  Right—left prevalence effect with horizontal and vertical effectors , 2004, Perception & psychophysics.

[9]  C. Iani,et al.  Facilitation and interference components in the joint Simon task , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[10]  N. Sebanz,et al.  Contextual determinants of the social-transfer-of-learning effect , 2011, Experimental Brain Research.

[11]  R. Proctor,et al.  Stimulus-Response Compatibility Principles: Data, Theory, and Application , 2006 .

[12]  Harold Bekkering,et al.  Action-effect binding by observational learning , 2011, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[13]  G. Aschersleben,et al.  The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): a framework for perception and action planning. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[14]  C. Iani,et al.  Attention control and susceptibility to hypnosis , 2009, Consciousness and Cognition.

[15]  A. Bandura Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory , 1985 .

[16]  J. J. Gibson The theory of affordances , 1977 .

[17]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: interpretations and misinterpretations , 2010, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[18]  D. Spalding The Principles of Psychology , 1873, Nature.

[19]  Marcel Brass,et al.  What is matched in direct matching? Intention attribution modulates motor priming. , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[20]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Mirror Neurons Differentially Encode the Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space of Monkeys , 2009, Science.

[21]  V. Gallese,et al.  Look What I Am Doing: Does Observational Learning Take Place in Evocative Task-Sharing Situations? , 2012, PloS one.

[22]  Gaetano Tieri,et al.  Where does an object trigger an action? An investigation about affordances in space , 2010, Experimental Brain Research.

[23]  M. Costantini,et al.  Tie my hands, tie my eyes. , 2012, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[24]  Sandro Rubichi,et al.  Shared learning shapes human performance: Transfer effects in task sharing , 2010, Cognition.

[25]  Johannes Dichgans,et al.  Motion habituation: Inverted self-motion perception and optokinetic after-nystagmus , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  V. Gallese,et al.  What is so special about embodied simulation? , 2011, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[27]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions. , 1996, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[28]  W. Prinz,et al.  Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. , 2001, Acta psychologica.

[29]  R. Passingham,et al.  Seeing or Doing? Influence of Visual and Motor Familiarity in Action Observation , 2006, Current Biology.

[30]  R. Shaw,et al.  Perceiving, Acting and Knowing : Toward an Ecological Psychology , 1978 .

[31]  Sandro Rubichi,et al.  Spatial coding in two dimensions , 2006, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[32]  R. Nicoletti,et al.  Co-occurrence of sequential and practice effects in the Simon task: Evidence for two independent mechanisms affecting response selection , 2009, Memory & cognition.

[33]  V. Gallese,et al.  Tool-use observation makes far objects ready-to-hand , 2011, Neuropsychologia.

[34]  R. Rescorla A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .

[35]  Andrew A G Mattar,et al.  Motor Learning by Observing , 2005, Neuron.

[36]  R. Proctor,et al.  Processing irrelevant location information: Practice and transfer effects in choice-reaction tasks , 1999, Memory & cognition.

[37]  W. Prinz Perception and Action Planning , 1997 .

[38]  G. Logan Toward an instance theory of automatization. , 1988 .

[39]  C. Heyes,et al.  Science Perspectives on Psychological Mirror Neuron Forum on Behalf Of: Association for Psychological Science , 2022 .

[40]  J. Mazziotta,et al.  Cortical mechanisms of human imitation. , 1999, Science.

[41]  Emily S. Cross,et al.  Robotic movement preferentially engages the action observation network , 2012, Human brain mapping.

[42]  Wolfgang Prinz,et al.  Action induction through action observation , 2004, Psychological research.

[43]  Marcel Brass,et al.  Contextual movement constraints of others modulate motor preparation in the observer , 2009, Neuropsychologia.