Perception of visual motion
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] R. Blake,et al. Brain activity evoked by inverted and imagined biological motion , 2001, Vision Research.
[2] G. Orban,et al. The kinetic occipital (KO) region in man: an fMRI study. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.
[3] O. Hikosaka,et al. Visual attention revealed by an illusion of motion , 1993, Neuroscience Research.
[4] Á. Pascual-Leone,et al. Fast Backprojections from the Motion to the Primary Visual Area Necessary for Visual Awareness , 2001, Science.
[5] G. Mather. The Movement Aftereffect and a Distribution-Shift Model for Coding the Direction of Visual Movement , 1980, Perception.
[6] D. Hubel,et al. Receptive fields and functional architecture of monkey striate cortex , 1968, The Journal of physiology.
[7] R. Blake,et al. The interplay between stereopsis and structure from motion , 1991, Perception & psychophysics.
[8] U. Ahlström,et al. Segregation of Motion Structure from Random Visual Noise , 1996, Perception.
[9] S. Exner,et al. Ueber optische Bewegungsempfindungen , 1888 .
[10] James E. McCarthy. Directional adaptation effects with contrast modulated stimuli , 1993, Vision Research.
[11] S. Tipper,et al. Priming reveals attentional modulation of human motion sensitivity , 1998, Vision Research.
[12] S. Shimojo,et al. When Sound Affects Vision: Effects of Auditory Grouping on Visual Motion Perception , 2001, Psychological science.
[13] S. Nishida,et al. Motion aftereffect with flickering test patterns reveals higher stages of motion processing , 1995, Vision Research.
[14] R. Blake,et al. Direction repulsion in motion transparency , 1996, Visual Neuroscience.
[15] D. Heeger. The Representation of Visual Stimuli in Primary Visual Cortex , 1994 .
[16] Karl R. Gegenfurtner,et al. Interaction of motion and color in the visual pathways , 1996, Trends in Neurosciences.
[17] M. Lappe,et al. Neuronal latencies and the position of moving objects , 2001, Trends in Neurosciences.
[18] E. Adelson,et al. The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .
[19] Robert Sekuler,et al. Detection and identification of moving targets , 1983, Vision Research.
[20] B. L. Gros,et al. Anisotropies in visual motion perception: a fresh look. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[21] V. Ramachandran,et al. Perceptual Organization in Multistable Apparent Motion , 1985, Perception.
[22] Jeffrey D. Schall,et al. Decision making: From sensory evidence to a motor command , 2000, Current Biology.
[23] D Jancke,et al. Orientation Formed by a Spot's Trajectory: A Two-Dimensional Population Approach in Primary Visual Cortex , 2000, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[24] Matthew C Smear,et al. Perception of Fourier and non-Fourier motion by larval zebrafish , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] R. Sekuler,et al. Mutual repulsion between moving visual targets. , 1979, Science.
[26] R. Sekuler,et al. Cues reduce direction uncertainty and enhance motion detection , 1981, Perception & psychophysics.
[27] W. Geisler. Sequential ideal-observer analysis of visual discriminations. , 1989 .
[28] A. Cowey,et al. Task–specific impairments and enhancements induced by magnetic stimulation of human visual area V5 , 1998, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[29] S. S. Stevens. Mathematics, measurement, and psychophysics. , 1951 .
[30] S. Yantis,et al. Perceptual grouping in space and time: Evidence from the Ternus display , 1997, Perception & psychophysics.
[31] R Sekuler,et al. How the Visual System Detects Changes in the Direction of Moving Targets , 1990, Perception.
[32] H. Barlow. The neuron doctrine in perception. , 1995 .
[33] S. Anand,et al. The selectivity and timing of motion processing in human temporo–parieto–occipital and occipital cortex: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study , 1998, Neuropsychologia.
[34] Christopher C. Pack,et al. Temporal dynamics of a neural solution to the aperture problem in visual area MT of macaque brain , 2001, Nature.
[35] G. DeAngelis,et al. Cortical area MT and the perception of stereoscopic depth , 1998, Nature.
[36] Brian J. Murphy,et al. Summation and discrimination of gratings moving in opposite directions , 1980, Vision Research.
[37] O. Braddick. Segmentation versus integration in visual motion processing , 1993, Trends in Neurosciences.
[38] D. Lewkowicz. Heterogeneity and heterochrony in the development of intersensory perception. , 2002, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[39] H Barlow,et al. Correspondence Noise and Signal Pooling in the Detection of Coherent Visual Motion , 1997, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[40] David J. Field,et al. Contour integration by the human visual system: Evidence for a local “association field” , 1993, Vision Research.
[41] R. Snowden. Suppressive interactions between moving patterns: Role of velocity , 1990, Perception & psychophysics.
[42] R Blake,et al. Another perspective on the visual motion aftereffect. , 1992, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[43] S. Hecht,et al. ENERGY, QUANTA, AND VISION , 1942, The Journal of general physiology.
[44] K. Dobkins. Moving Colors in the Lime Light , 2000, Neuron.
[45] Takeo Watanabe,et al. The role of parsing in high level motion processing , 1998 .
[46] G. Laurent,et al. Invariance of Angular Threshold Computation in a Wide-Field Looming-Sensitive Neuron , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[47] R. Andersen,et al. A Computational Framework for Determining Stereo Correspondence from a Set of Linear Spatial Filters Perception of Three-dimensional Structure from Motion Review , 2022 .
[48] R. Sekuler,et al. Detection of changes in speed and direction of motion: Reaction time analysis , 1993, Perception & psychophysics.
[49] James Campbell Quick,et al. Creating healthier workplaces: The American Psychological Association/National Institute of Occupational Safety and Health cooperative agreement. Introduction and historical overview. , 1997, Journal of occupational health psychology.
[50] J. Gold,et al. Neural computations that underlie decisions about sensory stimuli , 2001, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[51] S Ullman,et al. Maximizing Rigidity: The Incremental Recovery of 3-D Structure from Rigid and Nonrigid Motion , 1984, Perception.
[52] Michael von Grünau,et al. A motion aftereffect for long-range troboscopic apparent motion , 1986 .
[53] M. Corbetta,et al. Areas Involved in Encoding and Applying Directional Expectations to Moving Objects , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[54] S. Nishida,et al. Complete interocular transfer of motion aftereffect with flickering test , 1994, Vision Research.
[55] R A Andersen,et al. The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[56] H. Barlow. Vision: A theory about the functional role and synaptic mechanism of visual after-effects , 1991 .
[57] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Directional Motion Sensitivity under Transparent Motion Conditions , 1996, Vision Research.
[58] H. Barlow,et al. The statistical efficiency for detecting sinusoidal modulation of average dot density in random figures , 1981, Vision Research.
[59] I. M. Spigel. Contour absence as a critical factor in the inhibition of the decay of a movement aftereffect. , 1962 .
[60] N. Kanwisher,et al. Activation in Human MT/MST by Static Images with Implied Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[61] S. Watamaniuk. Ideal observer for discrimination of the global direction of dynamic random-dot stimuli. , 1993, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[62] C. Gilbert,et al. On a common circle: natural scenes and Gestalt rules. , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[63] E. Adelson,et al. Directionally selective complex cells and the computation of motion energy in cat visual cortex , 1992, Vision Research.
[64] A. Cowey,et al. Blindness to form from motion despite intact static form perception and motion detection , 2000, Neuropsychologia.
[65] T. Albright,et al. Image Segmentation Enhances Discrimination of Motion in Visual Noise , 1997, Vision Research.
[66] V. Ramachandran,et al. Visual inertia in apparent motion , 1987, Vision Research.
[67] M. Shiffrar,et al. Different motion sensitive units are involved in recovering the direction of moving lines , 1993, Vision Research.
[68] R. Sekuler,et al. Motion processing in peripheral vision: Reaction time and perceived velocity , 1982, Vision Research.
[69] R Sekuler,et al. Optic Flow Helps Humans Learn to Navigate through Synthetic Environments , 2000, Perception.
[70] K. Yu,et al. Can Semantic Knowledge Influence Motion Correspondence? , 2000, Perception.
[71] J. Gibson,et al. Motion parallax as a determinant of perceived depth. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[72] D. Lindsey. Direction repulsion in unfiltered and ring-filtered Julesz textures , 2001, Perception & psychophysics.
[73] Andrew B. Watson,et al. Window of visibility: a psychophysical theory of fidelity in time-sampled visual motion displays , 1986 .
[74] P. Thompson,et al. Human speed perception is contrast dependent , 1992, Vision Research.
[75] D. P. Gallogly,et al. Direction biasing by brief apparent motion stimuli , 2000, Vision Research.
[76] William H. Warren,et al. Optic flow is used to control human walking , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[77] Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al. Seeing motion behind occluders , 1995, Nature.
[78] S. Dakin,et al. The role of relative motion computation in ‘direction repulsion’ , 2000, Vision Research.
[79] C. Duffy,et al. Visual loss and getting lost in Alzheimer’s disease , 1999, Neurology.
[80] Myron L. Braunstein,et al. PERCEPTION OF MOTION , 1978 .
[81] R. Fox,et al. The perception of biological motion by human infants. , 1982, Science.
[82] G. Westheimer,et al. Discrimination of direction of motion in human vision. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.
[83] Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al. Direction Perception in Complex Dynamic Displays: the Integration of Dir~~tion Information , 1988 .
[84] J. Raymond. Complete interocular transfer of motion adaptation effects on motion coherence thresholds , 1993, Vision Research.
[85] R. Blake,et al. Interocular Transfer of Expansion, Rotation, and Translation Motion Aftereffects , 1994, Perception.
[86] Stefan Treue,et al. Different populations of neurons contribute to the detection and discrimination of visual motion , 2001, Vision Research.
[87] I. M. Spigel. THE USE OF DECAY INHIBITION IN AN EXAMINATION OF CENTRAL MEDIATION IN MOVEMENT AFTEREFFECTS. , 1964, The Journal of general psychology.
[88] G J Andersen. Perception of three-dimensional structure from optic flow without locally smooth velocity. , 1989, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[89] R. Sekuler,et al. Aftereffect of Seen Motion with a Stabilized Retinal Image , 1963, Science.
[90] Alexander Borst,et al. Models of motion detection , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.
[91] A. Rose. The sensitivity performance of the human eye on an absolute scale. , 1948, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[92] G. Francis,et al. Motion Parallel to Line Orientation: Disambiguation of Motion Percepts , 1999, Perception.
[93] R A Andersen,et al. Transparent motion perception as detection of unbalanced motion signals. I. Psychophysics , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[94] R. Blake,et al. Neural strength of visual attention gauged by motion adaptation , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[95] Robert H. Brown,et al. Visual sensitivity to differences in velocity. , 1961 .
[96] J. Lappin,et al. Coherence of early motion signals , 2001, Vision Research.
[97] A. Watson. Probability summation over time , 1979, Vision Research.
[98] Scott N. J. Watamaniuk,et al. Temporal and spatial integration in dynamic random-dot stimuli , 1992, Vision Research.
[99] G. Mather,et al. Gender discrimination in biological motion displays based on dynamic cues , 1994, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[100] J. Robson,et al. Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.
[101] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Movement aftereffect of bi-vectorial transparent motion , 1994, Vision Research.
[102] James W. Davis,et al. The Recognition of Human Movement Using Temporal Templates , 2001, IEEE Trans. Pattern Anal. Mach. Intell..
[103] D. Regan. Orientation discrimination for objects defined by relative motion and objects defined by luminance contrast , 1989, Vision Research.
[104] H. Barlow. The absolute efficiency of perceptual decisions. , 1980, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[105] H. Wallach,et al. The kinetic depth effect. , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[106] A. Watson,et al. The optimal motion stimulus , 1995, Vision Research.
[107] A. Cowey,et al. Perception of first‐ and second‐order motion: Separable neurological mechanisms? , 1999, Human brain mapping.
[108] D. Heeger,et al. Motion Opponency in Visual Cortex , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[109] R. Sekuler,et al. Hysteresis in the perception of motion direction as evidence for neural cooperativity , 1986, Nature.
[110] B. Albensi,et al. The Differential Optomotor Response of the Four-Eyed Fish Anableps Anableps , 1998, Perception.
[111] J. Rothwell,et al. Transcranial magnetic stimulation in cognitive neuroscience – virtual lesion, chronometry, and functional connectivity , 2000, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[112] T. Schenk,et al. Visual motion perception after brain damage: I. Deficits in global motion perception , 1997, Neuropsychologia.
[113] M. J. Wright,et al. Matching velocity in central and peripheral vision , 1986, Vision Research.
[114] T. Ledgeway. Adaptation to second-order motion results in a motion aftereffect for directionally-ambiguous test stimuli , 1994, Vision Research.
[115] Andrew P. Duchon,et al. The human visual system averages speed information , 1992, Vision Research.
[116] Preeti Verghese,et al. PII: S0042-6989(98)00033-9 , 1998 .
[117] M Nawrot,et al. A neural network model of kinetic depth , 1991, Visual Neuroscience.
[118] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Computational models of cortical visual processing. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[119] T. Pasternak,et al. Transient and permanent deficits in motion perception after lesions of cortical areas MT and MST in the macaque monkey. , 1999, Cerebral cortex.
[120] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Temporal and Spatial Frequency Tuning of the Flicker Motion Aftereffect , 1996, Vision Research.
[121] H. Barlow. The efficiency of detecting changes of density in random dot patterns , 1978, Vision Research.
[122] G. L. Zimmerman,et al. Evidence That Luminant and Equiluminant Motion Signals are Integrated by Directionally Selective Mechanisms , 1995, Perception.
[123] Wilson S. Geisler,et al. Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction , 1999, Nature.
[124] James E. Cutting,et al. Perception with an eye for motion , 1986 .
[125] N. Graham. Visual Pattern Analyzers , 1989 .
[126] Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos,et al. On the sufficiency of the velocity field for perception of heading , 1991, Biological Cybernetics.
[127] S. McKee. A local mechanism for differential velocity detection , 1981, Vision Research.
[128] O. Braddick,et al. Speed and direction of locally-paired dot patterns , 2000, Vision Research.
[129] A. Yonas,et al. Perception of three-dimensional shape specified by optic flow by 8-week-old infants , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.
[130] M. Rizzo,et al. Perception of movement and shape in Alzheimer's disease. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.
[131] W. Newsome,et al. Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects on direction discrimination performance , 1992, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[132] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Visual motion and the human brain: what has neuroimaging told us? , 2001, Acta psychologica.
[133] R. Blake,et al. Detection of temporal structure depends on spatial structure , 1999, Vision Research.
[134] Thomas D Albright,et al. Seeing the Big Picture Integration of Image Cues in the Primate Visual System , 1999, Neuron.
[135] F. Tong. Brain at work: play by play , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[136] P. Viviani,et al. The effect of movement velocity on form perception: Geometric illusions in dynamic displays , 1989, Perception & psychophysics.
[137] Robert Patterson,et al. Stereoscopic (cyclopean) motion sensing , 1999, Vision Research.
[138] S. Mateeff,et al. The simple reaction time to changes in direction of visual motion , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[139] A Pantle,et al. A multistable movement display: evidence for two separate motion systems in human vision. , 1976, Science.
[140] R. Blake,et al. Perception of coherent motion, biological motion and form-from-motion under dim-light conditions , 1999, Vision Research.
[141] R. Blake,et al. Memory for visual motion. , 1997, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[142] K. Tanaka,et al. Analysis of motion of the visual field by direction, expansion/contraction, and rotation cells clustered in the dorsal part of the medial superior temporal area of the macaque monkey. , 1989, Journal of neurophysiology.
[143] C. Baker,et al. Spatial frequency selective mechanisms underlying the motion aftereffect , 1992, Vision Research.
[144] M. Spetch,et al. Perception of coherent motion in random dot displays by pigeons and humans , 1999, Perception & psychophysics.
[145] George Sperling,et al. A Systems Analysis of Visual Motion Perception , 1999 .
[146] Claude L. Fennema,et al. Velocity determination in scenes containing several moving objects , 1979 .
[147] A. Pantle,et al. On the mechanism that encodes the movement of contrast variations: Velocity discrimination , 1989, Vision Research.
[148] S P Tripathy,et al. Insights into motion perception by observer modeling. , 1998, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[149] D. Burr,et al. Large receptive fields for optic flow detection in humans , 1998, Vision Research.
[150] W. Newsome,et al. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[151] S. McKee,et al. Temporal coherence theory for the detection and measurement of visual motion , 1995, Vision Research.
[152] P. Bennett,et al. Spatial phase differences can drive apparent motion , 1996, Perception & psychophysics.
[153] Lee Dn,et al. The optic flow field: the foundation of vision. , 1980 .
[154] Robert Sekuler,et al. Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.
[155] R. Blake,et al. Another means for measuring the motion aftereffect , 1993, Vision Research.
[156] David C. Burr,et al. Seeing biological motion , 1998, Nature.
[157] S. Yantis. Multielement visual tracking: Attention and perceptual organization , 1992, Cognitive Psychology.
[158] E. Wist,et al. The spatial frequency effect on perceived velocity , 1976, Vision Research.
[159] S. McKee,et al. Detecting a trajectory embedded in random-direction motion noise , 1995, Vision Research.
[160] V. Hömberg,et al. Cerebral visual motion blindness: transitory akinetopsia induced by transcranial magnetic stimulation of human area V5 , 1992, Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences.
[161] J. Maunsell,et al. Effects of Attention on the Processing of Motion in Macaque Middle Temporal and Medial Superior Temporal Visual Cortical Areas , 1999, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[162] T. Pasternak,et al. Microstimulation of cortical area MT affects performance on a visual working memory task. , 2001, Journal of neurophysiology.
[163] A. T. Smith,et al. The separability of temporal frequency and velocity , 1991, Vision Research.
[164] L A Riggs,et al. Visual aftereffects derived from inspection of orthogonally moving patterns. , 1980, Science.
[165] A. Parker,et al. Sense and the single neuron: probing the physiology of perception. , 1998, Annual review of neuroscience.
[166] K. Gegenfurtner,et al. Motion perception at scotopic light levels. , 2000, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[167] M N Shadlen,et al. Motion perception: seeing and deciding. , 1996, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.
[168] R. Sekuler,et al. The independence of channels in human vision selective for direction of movement. , 1975, The Journal of physiology.
[169] R. Sekuler,et al. Representational Development of Direction in Motion Perception: A Fragile Process , 1993, Perception.
[170] A. Derrington,et al. Detecting and discriminating the direction of motion of luminance and colour gratings , 1993, Vision Research.
[171] K. H. Britten,et al. Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision , 1989, Nature.
[172] G. Orban,et al. Factors influencing velocity coding in the human visual system , 1984, Vision Research.
[173] S. McKee,et al. Precise velocity discrimination despite random variations in temporal frequency and contrast , 1986, Vision Research.
[174] R O Dror,et al. Accuracy of velocity estimation by Reichardt correlators. , 2001, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[175] R. Blake. Cats Perceive Biological Motion , 1993 .
[176] R. Sekuler,et al. Assimilation and contrast in motion perception: Explorations in cooperativity , 1990, Vision Research.
[177] R. Sekuler,et al. Improving visual perception in older observers. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.
[178] K. Nakayama. Differential motion hyperacuity under conditions of common image motion , 1981, Vision Research.
[179] Mary J. Bravo,et al. Evidence for two speed signals: a coarse local signal for segregation and a precise global signal for discrimination , 1995, Vision Research.
[180] Irwin M. Spigel,et al. Relation of movement aftereffect duration to interpolated darkness intervals , 1962 .
[181] James T. Todd,et al. Opponent motion interactions , 1998 .
[182] P. Bennett,et al. Effects of aging on calculation efficiency and equivalent noise. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[183] G. Orban,et al. Motion-responsive regions of the human brain , 1999, Experimental Brain Research.
[184] Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al. ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain , 1994, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[185] Guy A. Orban,et al. Segregation of spatially superimposed optic flow components. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[186] J. Robson. Spatial and Temporal Contrast-Sensitivity Functions of the Visual System , 1966 .
[187] K. Nakayama,et al. Intact “biological motion” and “structure from motion” perception in a patient with impaired motion mechanisms: A case study , 1990, Visual Neuroscience.
[188] B. Frost,et al. Common reference frame for neural coding of translational and rotational optic flow , 1998, Nature.
[189] Stefan Treue,et al. Feature-based attention influences motion processing gain in macaque visual cortex , 1999, Nature.
[190] R. Sekuler,et al. Sound alters visual motion perception , 1997, Nature.
[191] G. Sperling,et al. Drift-balanced random stimuli: a general basis for studying non-Fourier motion perception. , 1988, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.
[192] A. W. Blackwell,et al. Perception of circular heading from optical flow. , 1991 .
[193] M. Chun,et al. Top-Down Attentional Guidance Based on Implicit Learning of Visual Covariation , 1999 .
[194] R. Sekuler,et al. Psychophysics of Motion Perception , 1982 .
[195] D G Pelli,et al. Why use noise? , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.
[196] P. Bennett,et al. Identification of band-pass filtered letters and faces by human and ideal observers , 1999, Vision Research.
[197] W. Newsome,et al. Neuronal and psychophysical sensitivity to motion signals in extrastriate area MST of the macaque monkey , 1994, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.
[198] H. Leibowitz. Effect of reference lines on the discrimination of movement. , 1955, Journal of the Optical Society of America.
[199] R. Blake,et al. Perception of Biological Motion , 1997, Perception.
[200] L. A. Symons,et al. The Aftereffect to Relative Motion Does Not Show Interocular Transfer , 1996, Perception.
[201] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Motion transparency: making models of motion perception transparent , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[202] S. Brownlow,et al. Perception of movement and dancer characteristics from point-light displays of dance , 1997 .
[203] Fred Attneave,et al. Apparent movement and the what-where connection. , 1974 .
[204] Z W Pylyshyn,et al. Tracking multiple independent targets: evidence for a parallel tracking mechanism. , 1988, Spatial vision.
[205] A. W. Blackwell,et al. Age differences in perceiving the direction of self-motion from optical flow. , 1989, Journal of gerontology.
[206] P. Thompson. Perceived rate of movement depends on contrast , 1982, Vision Research.
[207] T. Pasternak. Discrimination of differences in speed and flicker rate depends on directionally selective mechanisms , 1987, Vision Research.
[208] John Thorson,et al. Objective Measure of the Dynamics of a Visual Movement Illusion , 1969, Science.
[209] H. B. Barlow,et al. The precision of numerosity discrimination in arrays of random dots , 1983, Vision Research.
[210] G. Johansson. Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis , 1973 .
[211] Randolph Blake,et al. Detection and discrimination of optical flow components , 1998 .
[212] N. P. Bichot,et al. Reliability of Macaque Frontal Eye Field Neurons Signaling Saccade Targets during Visual Search , 2001, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[213] I. M. Spigel. The Effects of Differential Post-Exposure Illumination on the Decay of a Movement After-Effect , 1960 .
[214] R. Blake,et al. On the perceptual identity of dynamic stereopsis and kinetic depth , 1993, Vision Research.
[215] C. M. D. de Weert,et al. On Interocular Transfer of Motion Aftereffects , 1993, Perception.
[216] Frans A. J. Verstraten,et al. Recovery from adaptation for dynamic and static motion aftereffects: Evidence for two mechanisms , 1996, Vision Research.
[217] A. V. D. Berg,et al. Robustness of perception of heading from optic flow , 1992, Vision Research.
[218] H. Barlow,et al. Evidence for a Physiological Explanation of the Waterfall Phenomenon and Figural After-effects , 1963, Nature.
[219] H. Barlow,et al. The versatility and absolute efficiency of detecting mirror symmetry in random dot displays , 1979, Vision Research.
[220] T. Albright,et al. Efficient Discrimination of Temporal Patterns by Motion-Sensitive Neurons in Primate Visual Cortex , 1998, Neuron.
[221] David N. Lee,et al. A Theory of Visual Control of Braking Based on Information about Time-to-Collision , 1976, Perception.
[222] R. F. Wagner,et al. Efficiency of human visual signal discrimination. , 1981, Science.
[223] T. Pasternak,et al. The multiple roles of visual cortical areas MT/MST in remembering the direction of visual motion. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.
[224] P. Cavanagh,et al. Cortical fMRI activation produced by attentive tracking of moving targets. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.
[225] E. Leeuwenberg,et al. Vision: Space and movement: Volume 1, chapter 3, pp. 165–193 by Gerald Westheimer , 1990 .
[226] R. Shapley,et al. “On the Visually Perceived Direction of Motion” by Hans Wallach: 60 Years Later , 1996 .
[227] E. Adelson,et al. Phenomenal coherence of moving visual patterns , 1982, Nature.
[228] W. H. Warren,et al. Visual control of step length during running over irregular terrain. , 1986, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[229] T. Tayama. The Minimum Temporal Thresholds for Motion Detection of Grating Patterns , 2000, Perception.
[230] David Bradley. MT signals: better with time , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[231] M. Dawson,et al. The how and why of what went where in apparent motion: modeling solutions to the motion correspondence problem. , 1991, Psychological review.
[232] J. Raymond. Attentional modulation of visual motion perception , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[233] A F Bobick,et al. Movement, activity and action: the role of knowledge in the perception of motion. , 1997, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.
[234] Scott M. Dittman,et al. Monocular optical constraints on collision control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[235] J. Tresilian. Visually timed action: time-out for ‘tau’? , 1999, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[236] G. Orban,et al. Human velocity and direction discrimination measured with random dot patterns , 1988, Vision Research.
[237] S. McKee,et al. Simultaneous encoding of direction at a local and global scale , 1998, Perception & psychophysics.
[238] R. Blake,et al. Brain Areas Involved in Perception of Biological Motion , 2000, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[239] R. Snowden. Sensitivity to Relative and Absolute Motion , 1992, Perception.
[240] M. Greenlee,et al. Retention and disruption of motion information in visual short-term memory. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[241] Paul R. Schrater,et al. Perceiving visual expansion without optic flow , 2001, Nature.
[242] A. Pantle. Motion aftereffect magnitude as a measure of the spatio-temporal response properties of direction-sensitive analyzers. , 1974, Vision research.
[243] D. Burr,et al. A cortical area that responds specifically to optic flow, revealed by fMRI , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.