The "motion-blind" patient: low-level spatial and temporal filters

The “motion-blind” patient previously described by Zihl et al. (1983) was investigated using standard psychophysical procedures with stimuli whose spatial and temporal properties could be separately manipulated. Detection experiments for sinewave grating stimuli of varying spatial and temporal frequency showed sensitivity in this patient to be only slightly impaired. Temporal integration for stimuli of varying spatiotemporal frequency exhibited the expected space-time covariation seen in normal vision. An examination of the suprathreshold discriminative capacity of this patient was undertaken for spatial frequency, contrast, and temporal frequency. Although all of these discriminative functions were impaired, those concerning temporal frequency or velocity were dramatically reduced. No similar loss was seen for spatial frequency discrimination for moving or temporally varying stimuli. No measurable temporal frequency discrimination was present above 6 Hz and no velocity discrimination above 6 degrees/sec. Experiments involving the direction discrimination of suprathreshold drifting gratings of arrays of random dots revealed an inability to perceive direction of movement above a velocity of about 6 degrees/sec. Contrast thresholds contingent on direction of motion of a drifting grating also showed a much greater deficit than simple detection. Apparent motion using 2-flash random dot kinematograms revealed that the residual motion vision of this patient corresponded to the “short- range” motion process of normal vision. This process originally defined by Braddick (1974) operates over restricted space and time intervals. Apparent motion could only be supported by a narrow range of intermediate spatial displacements. These results suggest that this patient does exhibit some residual motion perception, probably corresponding to a severely impaired “short-range” mechanism. The patient's relatively intact ability to perform simple types of discrimination but severe impairment of performance at making judgments relevant to the nature of motion of the same stimuli suggests that while the components necessary for the analysis of motion are intact their more global associations have been disrupted. This implicates an extrastriate locus of the brain damage. Alternative explanations for the nature of the deficit are discussed.

[1]  Gordon T. Plant,et al.  Temporal frequency discrimination in human vision: Evidence for an additional mechanism in the low spatial and high temporal frequency region , 1985, Vision Research.

[2]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[3]  O E Favreau,et al.  Perceived velocity of moving chromatic gratings. , 1984, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[4]  Curtis L. Baker,et al.  A motion aftereffect from an isoluminant stimulus , 1985, Vision Research.

[5]  R. Hess,et al.  Differences in the neural basis of human amblyopia: The distribution of the anomaly across the visual field , 1985, Vision Research.

[6]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. II. Spatiotemporal range of directional interactions in MT and V1. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[7]  W. Newsome,et al.  Deficits in visual motion processing following ibotenic acid lesions of the middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey , 1985, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[8]  M. Cynader,et al.  Abolition of direction selectivity in the visual cortex of the cat. , 1976, Science.

[9]  O. Braddick A short-range process in apparent motion. , 1974, Vision research.

[10]  Curtis L. Baker,et al.  Eccentricity-dependent scaling of the limits for short-range apparent motion perception , 1985, Vision Research.

[11]  S. Zeki Colour coding in the cerebral cortex: The responses of wavelength-selective and colour-coded cells in monkey visual cortex to changes in wavelength composition , 1983, Neuroscience.

[12]  M. B. Mandler,et al.  Temporal frequency discrimination above threshold , 1984, Vision Research.

[13]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. III. Psychophysics and physiology of apparent motion. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  A. Watson,et al.  Patterns of temporal interaction in the detection of gratings , 1977, Vision Research.

[15]  P. D. Spear,et al.  Developmentally induced loss of direction-selective neurons in the cat's lateral suprasylvian visual cortex. , 1985, Brain research.

[16]  V. S. RAMACHANDRAN,et al.  Does colour provide an input to human motion perception? , 1978, Nature.

[17]  W. Newsome,et al.  Motion selectivity in macaque visual cortex. I. Mechanisms of direction and speed selectivity in extrastriate area MT. , 1986, Journal of neurophysiology.

[18]  S. Zeki Functional organization of a visual area in the posterior bank of the superior temporal sulcus of the rhesus monkey , 1974, The Journal of physiology.

[19]  S. Zeki Cortical projections from two prestriate areas in the monkey. , 1971, Brain research.

[20]  E. Adelson,et al.  The analysis of moving visual patterns , 1985 .

[21]  T. Pasternak,et al.  Pattern and motion vision in cats with selective loss of cortical directional selectivity , 1986, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[22]  D. Tolhurst,et al.  Reaction times in the detection of gratings by human observers: A probabilistic mechanism , 1975, Vision Research.

[23]  J. Robson,et al.  Discrimination at threshold: Labelled detectors in human vision , 1981, Vision Research.

[24]  S. Zeki,et al.  Response properties and receptive fields of cells in an anatomically defined region of the superior temporal sulcus in the monkey. , 1971, Brain research.

[25]  J. Robson Spatial and Temporal Contrast-Sensitivity Functions of the Visual System , 1966 .

[26]  N. Mai,et al.  Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. , 1983, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[27]  J Allman,et al.  Direction- and Velocity-Specific Responses from beyond the Classical Receptive Field in the Middle Temporal Visual Area (MT) , 1985, Perception.