The neural substrate of the ideomotor principle: An event-related fMRI analysis

The present fMRI study sought to investigate the neural basis of perceiving learned action effects and thereby to test for hypotheses based on the ideomotor principle. For this purpose, we had subjects undergo a two-phase experimental procedure comprising an acquisition and a test phase, the latter administered inside the MR scanner. During the acquisition phase, free-choice button presses were contingently followed by one of two tones of different pitch which thereby should become "learned action effects". During the following test phase, subjects were presented with the action effects either when in a passive (non-acting) state or when they carried out forced-choice button presses. Conform to our expectations, we found evidence for a motor effector activation following the passive perception of effect tones which elicited activation in the neural motor system (premotor and somatosensory cortices, SMA, and cerebellum). Surprisingly, however, this activation was observed for left-hand effect tones only, suggesting a basic asymmetry in the impact of ideomotor learning. Moreover, we found activation in the posterior prefrontal and temporo-parietal cortex in response to action effects during the pursuit of goal-directed action. This suggests that action effects attracted special attention and thereby engaged selective cognitive control processes to ensure task-appropriate performance. Finally, there was reduced premotor activation for response-compatible as compared to response-incompatible action effects which can be taken as indication for differential requirements on the motor system and thus for behavioral interference and/or facilitation by learned action effects.

[1]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Parceling of mesial frontal motor areas during ideation and movement using functional magnetic resonance imaging at 1.5 tesla , 1994, Annals of neurology.

[2]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Coloring an action: Intending to produce color events eliminates the Stroop effect , 2004, Psychological research.

[3]  B Hommel,et al.  The cognitive representation of action: Automatic integration of perceived action effects , 1996, Psychological research.

[4]  N. Cohen,et al.  Hippocampal system and declarative (relational) memory: Summarizing the data from functional neuroimaging studies , 1999, Hippocampus.

[5]  Peter Falkai,et al.  Neural mechanisms of advance preparation in task switching , 2006, NeuroImage.

[6]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Acquisition and Control of Voluntary Action — 1 Acquisition and Control of Voluntary Action , 2001 .

[7]  H Shibasaki,et al.  Invasive Recording of Movement‐Related Cortical Potentials in Humans , 1992, Journal of clinical neurophysiology : official publication of the American Electroencephalographic Society.

[8]  C. Frith,et al.  Experiencing Oneself vs Another Person as Being the Cause of an Action: The Neural Correlates of the Experience of Agency , 2002, NeuroImage.

[9]  Tom Beckers,et al.  Automatic integration of non-perceptual action effect features: the case of the associative affective Simon effect , 2002, Psychological research.

[10]  R. Passingham,et al.  Functional anatomy of the mental representation of upper extremity movements in healthy subjects. , 1995, Journal of neurophysiology.

[11]  M. Erb,et al.  fMRI Evaluation of Somatotopic Representation in Human Primary Motor Cortex , 2000, NeuroImage.

[12]  Elisabeth A. Murray,et al.  Role of the hippocampus plus subjacent cortex but not amygdala in visuomotor conditional learning in rhesus monkeys. , 1996, Behavioral neuroscience.

[13]  Jeffrey M. Zacks,et al.  Lateral somatotopic organization during imagined and prepared movements. , 2006, Journal of neurophysiology.

[14]  B. Hommel,et al.  Contiguity and contingency in action-effect learning , 2004, Psychological research.

[15]  K Kurata,et al.  Activation of the dorsal premotor cortex and pre-supplementary motor area of humans during an auditory conditional motor task. , 2000, Journal of neurophysiology.

[16]  G. Hammond,et al.  Correlates of human handedness in primary motor cortex: a review and hypothesis , 2002, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[17]  Leslie G. Ungerleider,et al.  Mechanisms of visual attention in the human cortex. , 2000, Annual review of neuroscience.

[18]  L. L. Porter,et al.  Morphological Characterization of a Cortico-cortical relay in the cat sensorimotor cortex. , 1997, Cerebral cortex.

[19]  A. Osman,et al.  Dimensional overlap: cognitive basis for stimulus-response compatibility--a model and taxonomy. , 1990, Psychological review.

[20]  Scott T. Grafton,et al.  Dorsal premotor cortex and conditional movement selection: A PET functional mapping study. , 1998, Journal of neurophysiology.

[21]  Herbert G. Vaughan,et al.  Relationship of neuronal activity to gross movement-related potentials in monkey pre- and postcentral cortex , 1977, Brain Research.

[22]  Michael Ziessler,et al.  Response-effect learning as a major component of implicit serial learning , 1998 .

[23]  T. Braver,et al.  Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of frequency, inhibition and errors. , 2001, Cerebral cortex.

[24]  B. Hommel,et al.  Effect anticipation and action control. , 2001, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[25]  Bernhard Hommel,et al.  Development of the acquisition and control of action-effect associations. , 2004, Acta psychologica.

[26]  T. Pons,et al.  Primary motor cortex receives input from area 3a in macaques , 1990, Brain Research.

[27]  M. Hallett,et al.  Cerebral Processes Related to Visuomotor Imagery and Generation of Simple Finger Movements Studied with Positron Emission Tomography , 1998, NeuroImage.

[28]  G. Rizzolatti,et al.  Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study , 2001, The European journal of neuroscience.

[29]  T. Egner,et al.  Cognitive control mechanisms resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant information , 2005, Nature Neuroscience.

[30]  Rainer Goebel,et al.  Attentional systems in target and distractor processing: a combined ERP and fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[31]  S P Wise,et al.  The premotor cortex and nonstandard sensorimotor mapping. , 1996, Canadian journal of physiology and pharmacology.

[32]  A. Meltzoff,et al.  A PET Exploration of the Neural Mechanisms Involved in Reciprocal Imitation , 2002, NeuroImage.

[33]  H. Tachibana,et al.  The role of the left somatosensory cortex in human hand movement , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[34]  H. Heuer,et al.  Perspectives on Perception and Action , 1989 .

[35]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[36]  Markus Schwaiger,et al.  A H2 15O Positron Emission Tomography Study on Mental Imagery of Movement Sequences—The Effect of Modulating Sequence Length and Direction , 2002, NeuroImage.

[37]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation-Induced Changes in Sensorimotor Coupling Parallel Improvements of Somatosensation in Humans , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[38]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[39]  B. Hommel The Quarterly Journal Of Experimental Psychology, 1996 , 49a (3) , 546 571 , 2022 .

[40]  W. James,et al.  The Principles of Psychology. , 1983 .

[41]  M. Brass,et al.  The role of the inferior frontal junction area in cognitive control , 2005, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[42]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Generalisability, Random Effects & Population Inference , 1998, NeuroImage.

[43]  Lutz Jäncke,et al.  Attention modulates activity in the primary and the secondary auditory cortex: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects , 1999, Neuroscience Letters.

[44]  P. Roland,et al.  I Feel My Hand Moving A New Role of the Primary Motor Cortex in Somatic Perception of Limb Movement , 2002, Neuron.

[45]  Stefan Geyer,et al.  Imagery of voluntary movement of fingers, toes, and tongue activates corresponding body-part-specific motor representations. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[46]  S. Zeki,et al.  Three cortical stages of colour processing in the human brain. , 1998, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[47]  J. Downar,et al.  A multimodal cortical network for the detection of changes in the sensory environment , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[48]  A. Kleinschmidt,et al.  Cross-Modal Processing in Early Visual and Auditory Cortices depends on Expected Statistical Relationship of Multisensory Information , 2006, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[49]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Multiple nonprimary motor areas in the human cortex. , 1997, Journal of neurophysiology.

[50]  E. Thorndike Ideo-motor action. , 1913 .

[51]  John C. Adair,et al.  The neural networks underlying endogenous auditory covert orienting and reorienting , 2006, NeuroImage.

[52]  Hartwig R. Siebner,et al.  Linking Actions and Their Perceivable Consequences in the Human Brain , 2002, NeuroImage.

[53]  Jean Decety,et al.  Leader or follower? Involvement of the inferior parietal lobule in agency , 2002, Neuroreport.

[54]  L. L. Porter,et al.  Patterns of connectivity in the cat sensory‐motor cortex: A light and electron microscope analysis of the projection arising from area 3a , 1991, The Journal of comparative neurology.

[55]  B. Hommel,et al.  Acquisition and generalization of action effects , 2003 .

[56]  Marc Jeannerod,et al.  Modulating the experience of agency: a positron emission tomography study , 2003, NeuroImage.

[57]  S P Wise,et al.  Role of the hippocampal system in associative learning beyond the spatial domain. , 2003, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  L. Jäncke,et al.  Tapping movements according to regular and irregular visual timing signals investigated with fMRI , 2000, Neuroreport.

[59]  D. Brooks,et al.  A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). , 1997, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[60]  Y. Yen,et al.  Deactivation of Sensory-Specific Cortex by Cross-Modal Stimuli , 2002, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[61]  A. Greenwald,et al.  Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: with special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. , 1970, Psychological review.

[62]  S. Kinomura,et al.  Regional cerebral blood flow changes of cortical motor areas and prefrontal areas in humans related to ipsilateral and contralateral hand movement , 1993, Brain Research.

[63]  H. Eichenbaum Hippocampus Cognitive Processes and Neural Representations that Underlie Declarative Memory , 2004, Neuron.

[64]  R. Desimone,et al.  Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. , 1995, Annual review of neuroscience.

[65]  C. Frith,et al.  Modulation of human visual cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. , 2000, Science.

[66]  S G Kim,et al.  Functional activation in motor cortex reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. , 1997, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[67]  Peter Fransson,et al.  Fingersomatotopy in area 3b: an fMRI-study , 2004, BMC Neuroscience.

[68]  J. Winn,et al.  Brain , 1878, The Lancet.

[69]  A. Holmes Generalisability, random effects and population inference (abstract) , 1998 .

[70]  O. Gruber,et al.  Oddball and incongruity effects during Stroop task performance: A comparative fMRI study on selective attention , 2006, Brain Research.

[71]  A Urbano,et al.  Human cortical activity related to unilateral movements. A high resolution EEG study , 1996, Neuroreport.

[72]  J. Downar,et al.  The Effect of Task Relevance on the Cortical Response to Changes in Visual and Auditory Stimuli: An Event-Related fMRI Study , 2001, NeuroImage.

[73]  M. Corbetta,et al.  Selective and divided attention during visual discriminations of shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission tomography , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[74]  M. Milham,et al.  Competition for priority in processing increases prefrontal cortex's involvement in top-down control: an event-related fMRI study of the stroop task. , 2003, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.

[75]  Ewald Moser,et al.  The selection of intended actions and the observation of others' actions: A time-resolved fMRI study , 2006, NeuroImage.