Assessing cross-modal target transition effects with a visual-auditory oddball.
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] E. John,et al. Evoked-Potential Correlates of Stimulus Uncertainty , 1965, Science.
[2] A. Kok. On the utility of P3 amplitude as a measure of processing capacity. , 2001, Psychophysiology.
[3] Erich Schröger,et al. Auditory distraction: event-related potential and behavioral indices , 2000, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[4] E. Schröger,et al. Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: a new distraction paradigm. , 1998, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[5] Arnaud Delorme,et al. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis , 2004, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[6] Jin Fan,et al. Testing the behavioral interaction and integration of attentional networks , 2009, Brain and Cognition.
[7] Joseph Dien,et al. Evaluating two-step PCA of ERP data with Geomin, Infomax, Oblimin, Promax, and Varimax rotations. , 2010, Psychophysiology.
[8] J. Polich,et al. Neuropsychology and neuropharmacology of P3a and P3b. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[9] John R. Anderson,et al. Relationship of P3b single-trial latencies and response times in one, two, and three-stimulus oddball tasks , 2017, Biological Psychology.
[10] R. C. Oldfield. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. , 1971, Neuropsychologia.
[11] N. Squires,et al. Two varieties of long-latency positive waves evoked by unpredictable auditory stimuli in man. , 1975, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[12] Christopher R. Brown,et al. Auditory processing in an inter-modal oddball task: effects of a combined auditory/visual standard on auditory target ERPs. , 2007, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[13] N. Birbaumer,et al. Prediction of Auditory and Visual P300 Brain-Computer Interface Aptitude , 2013, PloS one.
[14] C. Gonsalvez,et al. Target-to-target interval versus probability effects on P300 in one- and two-tone tasks. , 2003, Psychophysiology.
[15] S. Berti. The role of auditory transient and deviance processing in distraction of task performance: a combined behavioral and event-related brain potential study , 2013, Front. Hum. Neurosci..
[16] E. Donchin,et al. Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating? , 1988, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.
[17] R. Johnson. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude. , 1986, Psychophysiology.
[18] Daniel Strüber,et al. P300 and slow wave from oddball and single-stimulus visual tasks: inter-stimulus interval effects. , 2002, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[19] A. Coenen,et al. Neural generators of the auditory evoked potential components P3a and P3b. , 2012, Acta neurobiologiae experimentalis.
[20] Joseph Dien,et al. The ERP PCA Toolkit: An open source program for advanced statistical analysis of event-related potential data , 2010, Journal of Neuroscience Methods.
[21] D. Lehmann,et al. Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain. , 1994, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[22] Kiti Müller,et al. Relationship of P300 single-trial responses with reaction time and preceding stimulus sequence. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[23] E. Schröger,et al. A neural mechanism for involuntary attention shifts to changes in auditory stimulation. , 1996, Journal of cognitive neuroscience.
[24] Bruno H. Repp,et al. Auditory dominance in temporal processing: new evidence from synchronization with simultaneous visual and auditory sequences. , 2002, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[25] P. Andrés,et al. Distraction by deviance: comparing the effects of auditory and visual deviant stimuli on auditory and visual target processing. , 2015, Experimental psychology.
[26] Ray Johnson. For Distinguished Early Career Contribution to Psychophysiology: Award Address, 1985 , 1986 .
[27] T W Picton,et al. Temporal and sequential probability in evoked potential studies. , 1981, Canadian journal of psychology.
[28] Christopher R. Brown,et al. Inter-modal attention: ERPs to auditory targets in an inter-modal oddball task. , 2006, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[29] E. Schröger,et al. Bottom-up influences on working memory: behavioral and electrophysiological distraction varies with distractor strength. , 2004, Experimental psychology.
[30] J. Cohen,et al. On the number of trials needed for P300. , 1997, International journal of psychophysiology : official journal of the International Organization of Psychophysiology.
[31] J. Polich,et al. P3a and P3b from typical auditory and visual stimuli , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[32] I. Winkler,et al. Do N1/MMN, P3a, and RON form a strongly coupled chain reflecting the three stages of auditory distraction? , 2008, Biological Psychology.
[33] Amy M. Jimenez,et al. Impaired target detection in schizophrenia and the ventral attentional network: Findings from a joint event-related potential–functional MRI analysis , 2015, NeuroImage: Clinical.
[34] Ingmar H. A. Franken,et al. Internal Consistency of Event-Related Potentials Associated with Cognitive Control: N2/P3 and ERN/Pe , 2014, PloS one.
[35] Richard J. Harris,et al. Event-related potentials in cross-modal divided attention in autism , 1995, Neuropsychologia.
[36] F E Bloom,et al. P300 assessment of early Alzheimer's disease. , 1990, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[37] Bettina Sorger,et al. Novelty and target processing during an auditory novelty oddball: A simultaneous event-related potential and functional magnetic resonance imaging study , 2008, NeuroImage.
[38] Yuezhi Li,et al. Localizing P300 generators in high-density event- related potential with fMRI. , 2009, Medical science monitor : international medical journal of experimental and clinical research.
[39] E. Donchin. Presidential address, 1980. Surprise!...Surprise? , 1981, Psychophysiology.
[40] Daniel M. Roberts,et al. Contextual task difficulty modulates stimulus discrimination: electrophysiological evidence for interaction between sensory and executive processes. , 2012, Psychophysiology.
[41] Piotr Jaskowski,et al. Evidence for an Integrative Role of P3b in Linking Reaction to Perception , 2005 .
[42] D. Linden. The P300: Where in the Brain Is It Produced and What Does It Tell Us? , 2005, The Neuroscientist : a review journal bringing neurobiology, neurology and psychiatry.
[43] Thomas E. Nichols,et al. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples , 2002, Human brain mapping.
[44] K. Kiehl,et al. An event-related fMRI study of visual and auditory oddball tasks , 2001 .
[45] K L Shapiro,et al. Temporary suppression of visual processing in an RSVP task: an attentional blink? . , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[46] Babak A. Ardekani,et al. A functional MRI study of visual oddball: evidence for frontoparietal dysfunction in subjects at risk for alcoholism , 2004, NeuroImage.
[47] P Ullsperger,et al. P3 varies with stimulus categorization rather than probability. , 1993, Electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology.
[48] Rachel Rac-Lubashevsky,et al. Dissociating working memory updating and automatic updating: The reference-back paradigm. , 2016, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[49] E Gordon,et al. Is the target-to-target interval a critical determinant of P3 amplitude? , 1999, Psychophysiology.
[50] J. Polich. Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b , 2007, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[51] J. Polich,et al. P300 amplitude is determined by target-to-target interval. , 2002, Psychophysiology.
[52] A. Engel,et al. What is novel in the novelty oddball paradigm? Functional significance of the novelty P3 event-related potential as revealed by independent component analysis. , 2005, Brain research. Cognitive brain research.
[53] J. Polich,et al. Auditory and visual P300 topography from a 3 stimulus paradigm , 1999, Clinical Neurophysiology.
[54] S. Galderisi,et al. The cortical generators of P3a and P3b: A LORETA study , 2007, Brain Research Bulletin.
[55] M. Posner,et al. The attention system of the human brain. , 1990, Annual review of neuroscience.
[56] Phillip J. Holcomb,et al. The Dissociation between Early and Late Selection in Older Adults , 2013, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.