How to find the way out from four rooms? The learning of “chaining” associations may shed light on the neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia

Recent meta-analytic evidence suggests that clinical neuropsychological methods are not likely to uncover circumscribed cognitive impairments in the deficit syndrome of schizophrenia. To overcome this issue, we adapted a cognitive neuroscience perspective and used a new "chaining" habit learning task. Participants were requested to navigate a cartoon character through a sequence of 4 rooms by learning to choose the open door from 3 colored doors in each room. The aim of the game was to learn the full sequence of rooms until the character reached the outside. In the training phase, each stimulus leading to reward (open door in each room) was trained via feedback until the complete sequence was learned. In the probe phase, the context of rewarded stimuli was manipulated: in a given room, in addition to the correct door of that room, there also appeared a door which was open in another room. Whereas the training phase is dominantly related to basal ganglia circuits, the context-dependent probe phase requires intact medial-temporal lobe functioning. Results revealed that deficit and non-deficit patients were similarly impaired on the probe phase compared with controls. However, the training phase was only compromised in deficit patients. More severe negative symptoms were associated with more errors on the training phase. Executive functions were unrelated to performance on the "chaining" task. These results indicate that the deficit syndrome is associated with prominently impaired stimulus-response reinforcement learning, which may indicate abnormal functioning of basal ganglia circuits.

[1]  D. Sheehan,et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. , 1998, The Journal of clinical psychiatry.

[2]  R. Clark,et al.  The medial temporal lobe. , 2004, Annual review of neuroscience.

[3]  M. Yazıcı,et al.  Reexamination of the characteristics of the deficit schizophrenia patients , 2003, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[4]  Michael J. Frank,et al.  Dynamic Dopamine Modulation in the Basal Ganglia: A Neurocomputational Account of Cognitive Deficits in Medicated and Nonmedicated Parkinsonism , 2005, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Goldman,et al.  What is the functional significance of hippocampal pathology in schizophrenia? , 2004, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[6]  Holly Moore,et al.  The regulation of forebrain dopamine transmission: relevance to the pathophysiology and psychopathology of schizophrenia , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[7]  D. Weinberger,et al.  Cell biology of the hippocampal formation in schizophrenia , 1999, Biological Psychiatry.

[8]  A. Antal,et al.  Schizophrenics know more than they can tell: probabilistic classification learning in schizophrenia , 2000, Psychological Medicine.

[9]  A. Cohen,et al.  Neuropsychology of the deficit syndrome: new data and meta-analysis of findings to date. , 2007, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[10]  H. Yin,et al.  The role of the basal ganglia in habit formation , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[11]  H. Eichenbaum,et al.  Evolution of declarative memory , 2006, Hippocampus.

[12]  Catherine E. Myers,et al.  Dissociation between medial temporal lobe and basal ganglia memory systems in schizophrenia , 2005, Schizophrenia Research.

[13]  M. Gluck,et al.  The role of dopamine in cognitive sequence learning: evidence from Parkinson’s disease , 2005, Behavioural Brain Research.

[14]  M. Gluck,et al.  Cognitive sequence learning in Parkinson's disease and amnestic mild cognitive impairment: Dissociation between sequential and non-sequential learning of associations , 2007, Neuropsychologia.

[15]  S. Dollfus,et al.  Cognitive patterns in subtypes of schizophrenia , 2002, European Psychiatry.

[16]  D. Malaspina,et al.  Olfactory identification and WAIS-R performance in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia , 2003, Schizophrenia Research.

[17]  N. Alpert,et al.  Functional imaging of memory retrieval in deficit vs nondeficit schizophrenia. , 1999, Archives of general psychiatry.

[18]  Benjamin M. Robinson,et al.  Selective Reinforcement Learning Deficits in Schizophrenia Support Predictions from Computational Models of Striatal-Cortical Dysfunction , 2007, Biological Psychiatry.

[19]  H. Levin A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1993 .

[20]  T. Hergueta,et al.  The mini international neuropsychiatric interview , 1998, European Psychiatry.

[21]  B. Kirkpatrick,et al.  The heterogeneity of the long-term course of schizophrenia. , 1988, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[22]  J. Lieberman,et al.  Deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in schizophrenia: defining the pattern. , 2000, Archives of general psychiatry.

[23]  M. Gluck,et al.  Risk and protective haplotypes of the alpha‐synuclein gene associated with Parkinson’s disease differentially affect cognitive sequence learning , 2007, Genes, brain, and behavior.

[24]  Anna Juhász,et al.  Habit learning and the genetics of the dopamine D3 receptor: evidence from patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. , 2005, Behavioral Neuroscience.

[25]  C. Bell DSM-IV: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders , 1994 .

[26]  R. Buchanan,et al.  Neuropsychological impairments in deficit vs nondeficit forms of schizophrenia. , 1994, Archives of general psychiatry.

[27]  S. Heckers,et al.  Neuroimaging studies of the hippocampus in schizophrenia , 2001, Hippocampus.

[28]  Robert W. Buchanan,et al.  The schedule for the deficit syndrome: An instrument for research in schizophrenia , 1989, Psychiatry Research.

[29]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dopaminergic contribution to cognitive sequence learning , 2007, Journal of Neural Transmission.

[30]  R. Buchanan,et al.  A separate disease within the syndrome of schizophrenia. , 2001, Archives of general psychiatry.

[31]  Michael F. Green,et al.  Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia , 2004, Schizophrenia Research.

[32]  O. Spreen,et al.  A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary , 1991 .

[33]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dissociating Hippocampal versus Basal Ganglia Contributions to Learning and Transfer , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Brian Knutson,et al.  Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenic patients treated with typical, not atypical, neuroleptics , 2006, Psychopharmacology.

[35]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Prefrontal cortex dysfunction mediates deficits in working memory and prepotent responding in schizophrenia , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[36]  J BELBEY,et al.  What is schizophrenia? , 1982, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[37]  J. Malley,et al.  Habit and skill learning in schizophrenia: evidence of normal striatal processing with abnormal cortical input. , 2002, Learning & memory.

[38]  Jennifer A. Mangels,et al.  Typical and atypical antipsychotic medications differentially affect two nondeclarative memory tasks in schizophrenic patients: a double dissociation , 2003, Schizophrenia Research.

[39]  S. Kay,et al.  The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. , 1987, Schizophrenia bulletin.

[40]  E. Daneluzzo,et al.  Historical, psychopathological, neurological, and neuropsychological aspects of deficit schizophrenia: a multicenter study. , 2002, The American journal of psychiatry.

[41]  J. Danion,et al.  Functional mechanisms underlying impaired recognition memory and conscious awareness in patients with schizophrenia. , 1999, Archives of general psychiatry.

[42]  R Fog,et al.  A Progress Report , 2015 .