Reduction in direction discrimination with age and slow speed is due to both increased internal noise and reduced sampling efficiency.

PURPOSE Sensitivity to moving structure decreases with age and slow speeds may be selectively impaired. This loss could be caused by elevated internal noise in the responses of motion sensors or a reduction in the efficiency with which motion responses are integrated. We adapt an equivalent noise paradigm to analyze the perception of slow and fast speed motion as a function of normal aging. METHODS A total of 70 observers (20 to 89 years) identified the direction of global motion in a two-alternative forced choice task. In a central 8° aperture, 100 dots of 10% Michelson contrast were moving at 1.6 or 5.5°/s. The direction of each dot was drawn from a Gaussian distribution whose mean and SD were adaptively changed. Internal noise and sampling efficiency were estimated from direction discrimination thresholds as a function of external direction noise, speed, and age. RESULTS Direction sensitivity was significantly worse for slow speeds at all ages (paired t-test, P < 0.05) and decreased approximately 2% per year (linear regressions, P < 0.01). This aging deficit was due to significant changes in internal noise (5.5°/s) and sampling efficiency (1.6°/s) (linear regression, P < 0.05). CONCLUSIONS There is motion sensitivity loss with age that arises from an increase in internal noise in the responses of directional sensors and a decrease in responses that contribute to the global decision. Differences in the rates of progression at each speed indicate that motion is processed by independent systems tuned to different speeds, and that the channel for slow speed may be more vulnerable to normal age-related changes.

[1]  P. Atchley,et al.  The effect of age, retinal eccentricity, and speed on the detection of optic flow components. , 1998, Psychology and aging.

[2]  R A Andersen,et al.  The response of area MT and V1 neurons to transparent motion , 1991, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[3]  D C Van Essen,et al.  Functional properties of neurons in middle temporal visual area of the macaque monkey. I. Selectivity for stimulus direction, speed, and orientation. , 1983, Journal of neurophysiology.

[4]  Jing Liu,et al.  Functional organization of speed tuned neurons in visual area MT. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[5]  Allison B Sekuler,et al.  Spatial characteristics of center-surround antagonism in younger and older adults. , 2009, Journal of vision.

[6]  P. Henkind,et al.  Aging and degeneration of the human macula. 1. Outer nuclear layer and photoreceptors. , 1981, The British journal of ophthalmology.

[7]  Lisa R. Betts,et al.  The effects of aging on orientation discrimination , 2007, Vision Research.

[8]  Harriet A Allen,et al.  The role of contrast sensitivity in global motion processing deficits in the elderly. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[9]  R Sekuler,et al.  Visual fixation stability in older adults. , 1986, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[10]  M. Grassi,et al.  Positional noise in Landolt-C stimuli reduces spatial resolution: A study with younger and older observers , 2012, Vision Research.

[11]  R. Sekuler,et al.  The effects of aging on motion detection and direction identification , 2007, Vision Research.

[12]  J. Norman,et al.  Aging and the Perception of Speed , 2003, Perception.

[13]  S. Feldon,et al.  Age-related deterioration of motion perception and detection , 1998, Graefe's Archive for Clinical and Experimental Ophthalmology.

[14]  T. Stuve,et al.  Motion perception and aging. , 1992, Psychology and aging.

[15]  Christopher Patrick Taylor,et al.  Aging Reduces Center-Surround Antagonism in Visual Motion Processing , 2005, Neuron.

[16]  T. Brandt,et al.  Direction-specific impairment of motion perception and spatial orientation in downbeat and upbeat nystagmus in humans , 1998, Neuroscience Letters.

[17]  P. Bennett,et al.  Effects of aging on calculation efficiency and equivalent noise. , 1999, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[18]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Speed-Dependent Responses in V5: A Replication Study , 1999, NeuroImage.

[19]  Clara Casco,et al.  Effects of aging on visual contour integration and segmentation. , 2011, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[20]  E. Vul,et al.  Functional adaptive sequential testing. , 2010, Seeing and perceiving.

[21]  David R Badcock,et al.  Independent speed-tuned global-motion systems , 1998, Vision Research.

[22]  D G Pelli,et al.  The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: transforming numbers into movies. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[23]  P. Bennett,et al.  Spatiotemporal properties of apparent motion perception and aging. , 2010, Journal of vision.

[24]  R. Snowden,et al.  Motion Perception in the Ageing Visual System: Minimum Motion, Motion Coherence, and Speed Discrimination Thresholds , 2006, Perception.

[25]  J. Movshon,et al.  Sensitivity to visual motion in amblyopic macaque monkeys , 2006, Visual Neuroscience.

[26]  T. Ledgeway,et al.  Psychophysical correlates of global motion processing in the aging visual system: A critical review , 2012, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[27]  K. Gegenfurtner,et al.  Differential aging of motion processing mechanisms: Evidence against general perceptual decline , 2008, Vision Research.

[28]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Contrast sensitivity throughout adulthood , 1982, Vision Research.

[29]  C. Scialfa,et al.  Age differences in target identification as a function of retinal location and noise level: examination of the useful field of view. , 1987, Psychology and aging.

[30]  Shahina Pardhan,et al.  Contrast sensitivity loss with aging: sampling efficiency and equivalent noise at different spatial frequencies. , 2004, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.

[31]  Anthony J. Movshon,et al.  Visual Response Properties of Striate Cortical Neurons Projecting to Area MT in Macaque Monkeys , 1996, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[32]  D. Elliott,et al.  A comparison of sampling efficiency and internal noise level in young and old subjects , 1996, Vision Research.

[33]  Helle K. Falkenberg,et al.  Sources of motion-sensitivity loss in glaucoma. , 2007, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[34]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[35]  Helle K. Falkenberg,et al.  Sampling efficiency and internal noise for motion detection, discrimination, and summation , 2003, Vision Research.

[36]  D. Pelli The quantum efficiency of vision , 1990 .

[37]  H. Rodman,et al.  Coding of visual stimulus velocity in area MT of the macaque , 1987, Vision Research.

[38]  R. Sekuler,et al.  Improving visual perception in older observers. , 1986, Journal of gerontology.

[39]  Gregory C DeAngelis,et al.  Coding of horizontal disparity and velocity by MT neurons in the alert macaque. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[40]  H. Gao,et al.  Aging of the human retina. Differential loss of neurons and retinal pigment epithelial cells. , 1992, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[41]  K. Devaney,et al.  Neuron loss in the aging visual cortex of man. , 1980, Journal of gerontology.

[42]  G. Legge,et al.  Contrast discrimination in noise. , 1987, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics and image science.

[43]  W. Newsome,et al.  A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal visual area (MT) , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[44]  G. Trick,et al.  Visual sensitivity to motion , 1991, Neurology.

[45]  Steven C. Dakin,et al.  Local and global limitations on direction integration assessed using equivalent noise analysis , 2005, Vision Research.

[46]  Mark A. Bullimore,et al.  Changes in the lower displacement limit for motion with age. , 1995 .

[47]  K. Tanaka,et al.  Comparison of neuronal selectivity for stimulus speed, length, and contrast in the prestriate visual cortical areas V4 and MT of the macaque monkey. , 1994, Journal of neurophysiology.

[48]  D H Brainard,et al.  The Psychophysics Toolbox. , 1997, Spatial vision.

[49]  Allison M. McKendrick,et al.  Visual form perception from age 20 through 80 years. , 2013, Investigative ophthalmology & visual science.

[50]  S B Steinman,et al.  Topography of the age-related decline in motion sensitivity. , 1995, Optometry and vision science : official publication of the American Academy of Optometry.

[51]  R. Harwood Visual problems and falls. , 2001, Age and ageing.

[52]  Robert Sekuler,et al.  Coherent global motion percepts from stochastic local motions , 1984, Vision Research.

[53]  A. Leventhal,et al.  Degradation of stimulus selectivity of visual cortical cells in senescent rhesus monkeys , 2000, Nature Neuroscience.

[54]  J. Kulikowski,et al.  Bar-like S-cone stimuli reveal the importance of an intermediate temporal filter. , 2010, Journal of the Optical Society of America. A, Optics, image science, and vision.