The neural basis of social tactics: An fMRI study

One of the most powerful ways of succeeding in complex social interactions is to read the minds of companions and stay a step ahead of them. In order to assess neural responses to reciprocal mind reading in socially strained human relationships, we used a 3-T scanner to perform an event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging study in 16 healthy subjects who participated in the game of Chicken. Statistical parametric mapping showed that the counterpart effect (human minus computer) exclusively activated the medial frontal area corresponding to the anterior paracingulate cortex (PCC) and the supramarginal gyrus neighboring the posterior superior temporal sulcus (STS). Furthermore, when we analyzed the data to evaluate whether the subjects made risky/aggressive or safe/reconciliatory choices, the posterior STS showed that the counterpart had a reliable effect regardless of risky or safe decisions. In contrast, a significant opponent x selection interaction was revealed in the anterior PCC. Based on our findings, it could be inferred that the posterior STS and the anterior PCC play differential roles in mentalizing; the former serves as a general mechanism for mentalizing, while the latter is exclusively involved in socially risky decisions.

[1]  Andrea Peru,et al.  Impairment of executive functions in a patient with a focal lesion in the anterior cingulate cortex. Evidence from neuropsychological assessment. , 2004, Functional neurology.

[2]  Deanna M Barch,et al.  Opiate addicts lack error-dependent activation of rostral anterior cingulate , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[3]  Gregory G. Brown,et al.  Error Rate and Outcome Predictability Affect Neural Activation in Prefrontal Cortex and Anterior Cingulate during Decision-Making , 2002, NeuroImage.

[4]  T. Allison,et al.  Social perception from visual cues: role of the STS region , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[5]  S Thesen,et al.  Prospective acquisition correction for head motion with image‐based tracking for real‐time fMRI , 2000, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[6]  M. Torrens Co-Planar Stereotaxic Atlas of the Human Brain—3-Dimensional Proportional System: An Approach to Cerebral Imaging, J. Talairach, P. Tournoux. Georg Thieme Verlag, New York (1988), 122 pp., 130 figs. DM 268 , 1990 .

[7]  G. Wilkinson Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat , 1984, Nature.

[8]  M. Posner,et al.  Cognitive and emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[9]  Daniel Houser,et al.  A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[10]  T. Clutton‐Brock,et al.  Selfish sentinels in cooperative mammals. , 1999, Science.

[11]  Terry M. Peters,et al.  3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes , 1993, 1993 IEEE Conference Record Nuclear Science Symposium and Medical Imaging Conference.

[12]  C. Frith,et al.  Movement and Mind: A Functional Imaging Study of Perception and Interpretation of Complex Intentional Movement Patterns , 2000, NeuroImage.

[13]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[14]  G. Pagnoni,et al.  A Neural Basis for Social Cooperation , 2002, Neuron.

[15]  Michael Doebeli,et al.  Spatial structure often inhibits the evolution of cooperation in the snowdrift game , 2004, Nature.

[16]  R. Adolphs Cognitive neuroscience: Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour , 2003, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[17]  Takashi Hanakawa,et al.  Functional activity related to risk anticipation during performance of the Iowa gambling task , 2005, NeuroImage.

[18]  K. Zilles,et al.  Mind Reading: Neural Mechanisms of Theory of Mind and Self-Perspective , 2001, NeuroImage.

[19]  D. Dennett The Intentional Stance. , 1987 .

[20]  Matthew F S Rushworth,et al.  Functional Specialization within Medial Frontal Cortex of the Anterior Cingulate for Evaluating Effort-Related Decisions , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[21]  D I Perrett,et al.  Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions. , 1989, The Journal of experimental biology.

[22]  M. Hasselmo,et al.  The role of expression and identity in the face-selective responses of neurons in the temporal visual cortex of the monkey , 1989, Behavioural Brain Research.

[23]  M. Walton,et al.  Action sets and decisions in the medial frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[24]  Claire Hughes,et al.  Executive function in preschoolers: Links with theory of mind and verbal ability , 1998 .

[25]  D. I. Perrett,et al.  Visual and somatosensory processing in the macaque temporal cortex: the role of ‘expectation’ , 2004, Experimental Brain Research.

[26]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[27]  S. Baron-Cohen Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind , 1997 .

[28]  Christopher S. Monk,et al.  Choice selection and reward anticipation: an fMRI study , 2004, Neuropsychologia.

[29]  D. Perrett,et al.  Integration of form and motion in the anterior superior temporal polysensory area (STPa) of the macaque monkey. , 1996, Journal of neurophysiology.

[30]  Karl J. Friston,et al.  Statistical parametric maps in functional imaging: A general linear approach , 1994 .

[31]  T. Robbins,et al.  Dissociable aspects of performance on the 5-choice serial reaction time task following lesions of the dorsal anterior cingulate, infralimbic and orbitofrontal cortex in the rat: differential effects on selectivity, impulsivity and compulsivity , 2003, Behavioural Brain Research.

[32]  Marc A Schuckit,et al.  Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation , 2003, Biological Psychiatry.

[33]  N. Ramnani,et al.  Distinct portions of anterior cingulate cortex and medial prefrontal cortex are activated by reward processing in separable phases of decision-making cognition , 2004, Biological Psychiatry.

[34]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension , 1995, Cognition.

[35]  James K Rilling,et al.  The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions , 2004, NeuroImage.

[36]  S. Baron-Cohen,et al.  Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind” ? , 1985, Cognition.

[37]  J. Decety,et al.  The detection of intentional contingencies in simple animations in patients with delusions of persecution , 2003, Psychological Medicine.

[38]  C. Frith,et al.  Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks , 2000, Neuropsychologia.

[39]  L. Moses,et al.  The role of inhibitory processes in young children's difficulties with deception and false belief. , 1998, Child development.

[40]  Douglas Frye,et al.  Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning , 1995 .

[41]  Christopher D. Frith,et al.  Imaging the Intentional Stance in a Competitive Game , 2002, NeuroImage.

[42]  H. Brownell,et al.  Acquired `theory of mind' impairments following stroke , 1999, Cognition.

[43]  D I Perrett,et al.  Organization and functions of cells responsive to faces in the temporal cortex. , 1992, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[44]  R Turner,et al.  Optimized EPI for fMRI studies of the orbitofrontal cortex , 2003, NeuroImage.

[45]  R. Adolphs The neurobiology of social cognition , 2001, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[46]  R. Knight,et al.  Frontal Lobe Contributions to Theory of Mind , 1998, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[47]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[48]  J. Neumann,et al.  Prisoner's Dilemma , 1993 .

[49]  Aina Puce,et al.  Electrophysiology and brain imaging of biological motion. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[50]  M. Walton,et al.  The Role of Rat Medial Frontal Cortex in Effort-Based Decision Making , 2002, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[51]  Tim Clutton-Brock,et al.  Breeding together: kin selection and mutualism in cooperative vertebrates. , 2002, Science.

[52]  C. Frith,et al.  Exploring ‘theory of mind’ in people with schizophrenia , 1996, Psychological Medicine.

[53]  Matthew D. Lieberman,et al.  Why rejection hurts: a common neural alarm system for physical and social pain , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  G. McCarthy,et al.  When Strangers Pass , 2004, Psychological science.

[55]  J. Decety,et al.  A PET Investigation of the Attribution of Intentions with a Nonverbal Task , 2000, NeuroImage.

[56]  A. Dale,et al.  Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex: A role in reward-based decision making , 2001, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[57]  D. Stuss,et al.  The frontal lobes are necessary for 'theory of mind'. , 2001, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[58]  Andrew N. Meltzoff,et al.  The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation , 2004, NeuroImage.

[59]  D. Premack,et al.  Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? , 1978, Behavioral and Brain Sciences.

[60]  E. Bullmore,et al.  Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study , 1999, The European journal of neuroscience.