Bayesian Behavioral Systems Theory
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] H. B. Barlow,et al. Possible Principles Underlying the Transformations of Sensory Messages , 2012 .
[2] D. Knill,et al. The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation , 2004, Trends in Neurosciences.
[3] C. Gallistel,et al. Risk assessment in man and mouse , 2009, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[4] Ryan D Ward,et al. It's the information! , 2013, Behavioural Processes.
[5] R. Rescorla. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: The effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement , 1972 .
[6] Patrick Simen,et al. Optimal response rates in humans and rats. , 2015, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal learning and cognition.
[7] W. Timberlake,et al. Stimulus and response contingencies in the misbehavior of rats. , 1982, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[8] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Cardinal rules: Visual orientation perception reflects knowledge of environmental statistics , 2011, Nature Neuroscience.
[9] R. Church,et al. A modular theory of learning and performance , 2007, Psychonomic bulletin & review.
[10] R M Church,et al. Scalar Timing in Memory , 1984, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences.
[11] W. Timberlake,et al. Auto-Shaping in Rats to the Presentation of Another Rat Predicting Food , 1975, Science.
[12] C. Gallistel,et al. Time to rethink the neural mechanisms of learning and memory , 2014, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory.
[13] Wei Ji Ma,et al. Bayesian inference with probabilistic population codes , 2006, Nature Neuroscience.
[14] Joseph T. McGuire,et al. A Neural Signature of Hierarchical Reinforcement Learning , 2011, Neuron.
[15] R. Church,et al. Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[16] P. Dayan,et al. Opinion TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences Vol.10 No.8 Full text provided by www.sciencedirect.com A normative perspective on motivation , 2022 .
[17] Michael N. Shadlen,et al. Temporal context calibrates interval timing , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.
[18] John H. Wearden,et al. Changing Sensitivity to Duration in Human Scalar Timing: An Experiment, a Review, and Some Possible Explanations , 1997 .
[19] Carlos Diuk,et al. Hierarchical Learning Induces Two Simultaneous, But Separable, Prediction Errors in Human Basal Ganglia , 2013, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[20] Within-session modulation of timed anticipatory responding: When to start responding , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[21] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. Optimal Temporal Risk Assessment , 2011, Front. Integr. Neurosci..
[22] W. E. Hick. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 1948, Nature.
[23] Tuğçe Tosun,et al. Mice plan decision strategies based on previously learned time intervals, locations, and probabilities , 2016, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[24] Stephen G Lisberger,et al. Neural implementation of Bayesian inference in a sensory-motor behavior , 2018, Nature Neuroscience.
[25] Alec Solway,et al. Optimal Behavioral Hierarchy , 2014, PLoS Comput. Biol..
[26] John Garcia,et al. Relation of cue to consequence in avoidance learning , 1966 .
[27] C R Gallistel,et al. Finding numbers in the brain , 2018, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[28] Russell M Church,et al. Optimal timing , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[29] W. Timberlake. Rats’ responses to a moving object related to food or water: A behavior-systems analysis , 1983 .
[30] Kathleen M. Silva,et al. The organization and temporal properties of appetitive behavior in rats , 1998 .
[31] M. Botvinick. Hierarchical reinforcement learning and decision making , 2012, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.
[32] Bilgehan Çavdaroğlu,et al. Time-based reward maximization , 2014, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences.
[33] C. Gallistel,et al. Temporal maps and informativeness in associative learning , 2009, Trends in Neurosciences.
[34] C. Gallistel,et al. Mice take calculated risks , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.
[35] Marcia L. Spetch,et al. Reward magnitude and timing in pigeons , 2011, Behavioural Processes.
[37] A. Pouget,et al. Probabilistic brains: knowns and unknowns , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[38] P. Dayan,et al. Tonic dopamine: opportunity costs and the control of response vigor , 2007, Psychopharmacology.
[39] M. Botvinick,et al. Neural representations of events arise from temporal community structure , 2013, Nature Neuroscience.
[40] The importance of the reinforcer as a time marker , 2010, Behavioural Processes.
[41] Chrystopher L. Nehaniv,et al. Hierarchical Behaviours: Getting the Most Bang for Your Bit , 2009, ECAL.
[42] Eero P. Simoncelli,et al. Origin and Function of Tuning Diversity in Macaque Visual Cortex , 2015, Neuron.
[43] G. Pezzulo,et al. An information-theoretic perspective on the costs of cognition , 2018, Neuropsychologia.
[44] Patrick Simen,et al. A decision model of timing , 2016, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.
[45] H. Rubenstein,et al. Test of Response Bias Explanation of Word-Frequency Effect , 1961, Science.
[46] C. Gallistel,et al. Interval timing in genetically modified mice: a simple paradigm , 2008, Genes, brain, and behavior.
[47] Abraham Wald,et al. Statistical Decision Functions , 1951 .
[48] K. Breland,et al. The misbehavior of organisms. , 1961 .
[49] Richard S. Sutton,et al. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.
[50] B. Campbell,et al. The role of experience in the spontaneous activity of hungry rats. , 1954, Journal of comparative and physiological psychology.
[51] Fuat Balci,et al. Motivational effects on interval timing in dopamine transporter (DAT) knockdown mice , 2010, Brain Research.
[52] J. Horvitz,et al. Amphetamine affects the start of responding in the peak interval timing task , 2007, Behavioural Processes.
[53] P. Dayan,et al. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors , 2011, Neuron.
[54] G. A. Miller. THE PSYCHOLOGICAL REVIEW THE MAGICAL NUMBER SEVEN, PLUS OR MINUS TWO: SOME LIMITS ON OUR CAPACITY FOR PROCESSING INFORMATION 1 , 1956 .
[55] Jiqiang Guo,et al. Stan: A Probabilistic Programming Language. , 2017, Journal of statistical software.
[56] Daniel Polani,et al. Grounding subgoals in information transitions , 2011, 2011 IEEE Symposium on Adaptive Dynamic Programming and Reinforcement Learning (ADPRL).
[57] Kathleen M. Silva,et al. A Behavior Systems View of Conditioned States during Long and Short CS-US Intervals , 1997 .
[58] F. Balcı. Interval Timing, Dopamine, and Motivation , 2014 .
[59] C R Gallistel,et al. Theoretical implications of quantitative properties of interval timing and probability estimation in mouse and rat. , 2017, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[60] Jonathan D. Cohen,et al. A Model of Interval Timing by Neural Integration , 2011, The Journal of Neuroscience.
[61] E. Kyonka,et al. Choice and timing in pigeons under differing levels of food deprivation , 2014, Behavioural Processes.
[62] F. Balcı,et al. Probabilistic Information Modulates the Timed Response Inhibition Deficit in Aging Mice , 2019, Front. Behav. Neurosci..
[63] Andrew T. Marshall,et al. Motivation and timing: Clues for modeling the reward system , 2012, Behavioural Processes.
[64] S S Stevens,et al. To Honor Fechner and Repeal His Law: A power function, not a log function, describes the operating characteristic of a sensory system. , 1961, Science.
[65] M. A. Girshick,et al. Theory of games and statistical decisions , 1955 .
[66] D. M. Green,et al. Signal detection theory and psychophysics , 1966 .
[67] Peter Sterling,et al. Principles of Neural Design , 2015 .
[68] Daniel A. Gottlieb. Is the number of trials a primary determinant of conditioned responding? , 2008, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[69] C. Sims. Rate–distortion theory and human perception , 2016, Cognition.
[70] R. Rescorla,et al. Within-subject effects of number of trials in rat conditioning procedures. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes.
[71] P. Killeen,et al. A behavioral theory of timing. , 1988, Psychological review.
[72] Thomas G. Dietterich. Hierarchical Reinforcement Learning with the MAXQ Value Function Decomposition , 1999, J. Artif. Intell. Res..
[73] Doina Precup,et al. Between MDPs and Semi-MDPs: A Framework for Temporal Abstraction in Reinforcement Learning , 1999, Artif. Intell..
[74] F. Balcı,et al. Sex differences in the timing behavior performance of 3xTg-AD and wild-type mice in the peak interval procedure , 2019, Behavioural Brain Research.
[75] Warren H. Meck,et al. Bayesian optimization of time perception , 2013, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[76] A. Machado. Learning the temporal dynamics of behavior. , 1997, Psychological review.
[77] Peter Dayan,et al. How fast to work: Response vigor, motivation and tonic dopamine , 2005, NIPS.
[78] Mauro Barni,et al. An Information Theoretic Perspective , 2004 .
[79] Wei Ji Ma,et al. Probabilistic brains: knowns and , 2013 .
[80] William Timberlake,et al. Motivational modes in behavior systems , 2000 .
[81] Self-Concept Variables. Sex Differences in , 2016 .
[82] Sarah Marzen,et al. The evolution of lossy compression , 2015, Journal of The Royal Society Interface.
[83] F. Attneave. Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.
[84] J. T. Erichsen,et al. Optimal prey selection in the great tit (Parus major) , 1977, Animal Behaviour.
[85] J. Gibbon. Scalar expectancy theory and Weber's law in animal timing. , 1977 .
[86] H. M. Jenkins,et al. The form of the auto-shaped response with food or water reinforcers. , 1973, Journal of the experimental analysis of behavior.
[87] Ning Qian,et al. Neuronal Firing Rate As Code Length: a Hypothesis , 2020, Computational Brain & Behavior.
[88] L. Sackney,et al. Contemporary Learning Theories, Instructional Design and Leadership , 2007 .
[89] Patrick Simen,et al. Decision processes in temporal discrimination. , 2014, Acta psychologica.