Neural systems for guilt from actions affecting self versus others

Guilt is a core emotion governing social behavior by promoting compliance with social norms or self-imposed standards. The goal of this study was to contrast guilty responses to actions that affect self versus others, since actions with social consequences are hypothesized to yield greater guilty feelings due to adopting the perspective and subjective emotional experience of others. Sixteen participants were presented with brief hypothetical scenarios in which the participant's actions resulted in harmful consequences to self (guilt-self) or to others (guilt-other) during functional MRI. Participants felt more intense guilt for guilt-other than guilt-self and guilt-neutral scenarios. Guilt scenarios revealed distinct regions of activity correlated with intensity of guilt, social consequences of actions, and the interaction of guilt by social consequence. Guilt intensity was associated with activation of the dorsomedial PFC, superior frontal gyrus, supramarginal gyrus, and anterior inferior frontal gyrus. Guilt accompanied by social consequences was associated with greater activation than without social consequences in the ventromedial and dorsomedial PFC, precuneus, posterior cingulate, and posterior superior temporal sulcus. Finally, the interaction analysis highlighted select regions that were more strongly correlated with guilt intensity as a function of social consequence, including the left anterior inferior frontal gyrus, left ventromedial PFC, and left anterior inferior parietal cortex. Our results suggest these regions intensify guilt where harm to others may incur a greater social cost.

[1]  J. O'Doherty,et al.  Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others , 2006, Nature.

[2]  C. Frith,et al.  Interacting minds--a biological basis. , 1999, Science.

[3]  C. Batson Two forms of perspective taking: Imagining how another feels and imagining how you would feel. , 2008 .

[4]  Christian Keysers,et al.  Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex , 2007, NeuroImage.

[5]  Matthew A. Lambon Ralph,et al.  Selective functional integration between anterior temporal and distinct fronto-mesolimbic regions during guilt and indignation , 2010, NeuroImage.

[6]  Tetsuya Matsuda,et al.  Brain activation associated with evaluative processes of guilt and embarrassment: an fMRI study , 2004, NeuroImage.

[7]  Stephen M Smith,et al.  Fast robust automated brain extraction , 2002, Human brain mapping.

[8]  A. Damasio,et al.  Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. , 2000, Cerebral cortex.

[9]  N. Alpert,et al.  Activation of anterior paralimbic structures during guilt-related script-driven imagery , 2000, Biological Psychiatry.

[10]  Rebecca Saxe,et al.  Overlapping and non-overlapping brain regions for theory of mind and self reflection in individual subjects. , 2006, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[11]  Griselda J. Garrido,et al.  The neural basis of human social values: evidence from functional MRI. , 2009, Cerebral cortex.

[12]  S M Smith,et al.  Overview of fMRI analysis. , 2004, The British journal of radiology.

[13]  M. Hoffman 11 – Development of Prosocial Motivation: Empathy and Guilt , 1982 .

[14]  Michael Brady,et al.  Improved Optimization for the Robust and Accurate Linear Registration and Motion Correction of Brain Images , 2002, NeuroImage.

[15]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Dissociable Medial Prefrontal Contributions to Judgments of Similar and Dissimilar Others , 2006, Neuron.

[16]  J. Tangney,et al.  Conceptual and methodological issues in the assessment of shame and guilt. , 1996, Behaviour research and therapy.

[17]  C. Neil Macrae,et al.  Perspective-Taking from a Social Neuroscience Standpoint , 2008 .

[18]  Richard S. J. Frackowiak,et al.  Deontological and altruistic guilt: Evidence for distinct neurobiological substrates , 2011, Human brain mapping.

[19]  Jorge Moll,et al.  Subgenual cingulate activity reflects individual differences in empathic concern , 2009, Neuroscience Letters.

[20]  Ingrid R. Olson,et al.  Social cognition and the anterior temporal lobes , 2010, NeuroImage.

[21]  R. Adolphs,et al.  Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements , 2007, Nature.

[22]  Joan Y. Chiao,et al.  Neural representations of social status hierarchy in human inferior parietal cortex , 2009, Neuropsychologia.

[23]  S. Preston,et al.  Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. , 2001, The Behavioral and brain sciences.

[24]  S. Carey,et al.  Understanding other minds: linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. , 2004, Annual review of psychology.

[25]  B. Maxwell Personality, identity, and character: explorations in moral psychology , 2011 .

[26]  Claus Lamm,et al.  The Neural Substrate of Human Empathy: Effects of Perspective-taking and Cognitive Appraisal , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[27]  M. Iacoboni,et al.  Getting a grip on other minds: Mirror neurons, intention understanding, and cognitive empathy , 2006, Social Neuroscience.

[28]  R Saxe,et al.  People thinking about thinking people The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind” , 2003, NeuroImage.

[29]  Giorgio Coricelli,et al.  Response to Comment on "The Involvement of the Orbitofrontal Cortex in the Experience of Regret" , 2005, Science.

[30]  Benedikt Szmrecsanyi,et al.  On operationalizing syntactic complexity , 2004 .

[31]  Jon R. Webb,et al.  Gender Differences in the Relationship Between Empathy and Forgiveness , 2005, The Journal of social psychology.

[32]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  The self as a moral agent: Linking the neural bases of social agency and moral sensitivity , 2007, Social neuroscience.

[33]  Michèle Wessa,et al.  An Agent Harms a Victim: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Study on Specific Moral Emotions , 2008, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[34]  Uta Frith,et al.  Theory of mind , 2001, Current Biology.

[35]  J. Moll,et al.  Neuroscience and Morality: Moral judgments, sentiments and values , 2008 .

[36]  Gereon R. Fink,et al.  Mirror Neuron and Theory of Mind Mechanisms Involved in Face-to-Face Interactions: A Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Approach to Empathy , 2007, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[37]  C. Frith,et al.  Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. , 2003, Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological sciences.

[38]  J. Grafman,et al.  The neural basis of human moral cognition , 2005, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[39]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Social conceptual impairments in frontotemporal lobar degeneration with right anterior temporal hypometabolism. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[40]  M. Buonocore,et al.  Posterior cingulate cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task , 2003, Human brain mapping.

[41]  J. Haidt,et al.  The Moral Emotions , 2009 .

[42]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Improved Assessment of Significant Activation in Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI): Use of a Cluster‐Size Threshold , 1995, Magnetic resonance in medicine.

[43]  Jordan Grafman,et al.  Social concepts are represented in the superior anterior temporal cortex , 2007, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[44]  John Suckling,et al.  Shared Neural Circuits for Mentalizing about the Self and Others , 2010, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[45]  J. Aharon-Peretz,et al.  Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. , 2009, Brain : a journal of neurology.

[46]  Emery N. Brown,et al.  Distinct Regions of Right Temporo-Parietal Junction Are Selective for Theory of Mind and Exogenous Attention , 2009, PloS one.

[47]  Jordan Henry Grafman,et al.  Impairment of prosocial sentiments is associated with frontopolar and septal damage in frontotemporal dementia , 2011, NeuroImage.

[48]  J. Decety,et al.  The functional architecture of human empathy. , 2004, Behavioral and cognitive neuroscience reviews.

[49]  Dana Samson,et al.  Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else's belief , 2004, Nature Neuroscience.

[50]  J. Decety,et al.  How Would You Feel versus How Do You Think She Would Feel? A Neuroimaging Study of Perspective-Taking with Social Emotions , 2004, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[51]  Jason P. Mitchell,et al.  Taking Another Person's Perspective Increases Self-Referential Neural Processing , 2008, Psychological science.

[52]  F. D. Waal Putting the Altruism Back into Altruism: The Evolution of Empathy , 2008 .

[53]  Joshua D. Greene,et al.  How (and where) does moral judgment work? , 2002, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[54]  Paul J. Eslinger,et al.  Neurological and Neuropsychological Bases of Empathy , 1998, European Neurology.

[55]  J. Tangney,et al.  Shame and Guilt , 2002 .

[56]  Jorge Moll,et al.  Moral judgments, emotions and the utilitarian brain , 2007, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[57]  C. Frith,et al.  Meeting of minds: the medial frontal cortex and social cognition , 2006, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[58]  Stephen M. Smith,et al.  A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images , 2001, Medical Image Anal..

[59]  J. Grafman,et al.  Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition. , 2005, Nature reviews. Neuroscience.