Understanding patch foraging strategies across development

[1]  A. Bornstein,et al.  Overharvesting in human patch foraging reflects rational structure learning and adaptive planning , 2023, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[2]  G. Zacharopoulos,et al.  Dissecting the neurocomputational bases of patch-switching , 2023, Cerebral cortex.

[3]  Marco K. Wittmann,et al.  Local and global reward learning in the lateral frontal cortex show differential development during human adolescence , 2023, PLoS biology.

[4]  A. Villringer,et al.  Diminished reinforcement sensitivity in adolescence is associated with enhanced response switching and reduced coding of choice probability in the medial frontal pole , 2023, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[5]  M. Munafo,et al.  Childhood maltreatment and mental health problems: A systematic review and meta-analysis of quasi-experimental studies , 2023, The American journal of psychiatry.

[6]  L. Schwabe,et al.  Disentangling the roles of dopamine and noradrenaline in the exploration-exploitation tradeoff during human decision-making , 2022, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[7]  Jonathan D. Cohen,et al.  Effort Foraging Task reveals positive correlation between individual differences in the cost of cognitive and physical effort in humans and relationship to self-reported motivation and affect , 2022, bioRxiv.

[8]  J. Raduà,et al.  Psychological trauma as a transdiagnostic risk factor for mental disorder: an umbrella meta-analysis , 2022, European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience.

[9]  Emma J. Kilford,et al.  Confirmatory reinforcement learning changes with age during adolescence. , 2022, Developmental science.

[10]  Adrián R. Muñoz-García,et al.  Quitting rules in hybrid foraging search: From early childhood to early adulthood , 2022, Cognitive Development.

[11]  Catherine A. Hartley,et al.  Real-World Exploration Increases Across Adolescence and Relates to Affect, Risk Taking, and Social Connectivity , 2022, Psychology Science.

[12]  S. Eickhoff,et al.  Risk‐taking in the human brain: An activation likelihood estimation meta‐analysis of the balloon analog risk task (BART) , 2022, Human brain mapping.

[13]  T. Hauser,et al.  Value-free random exploration is linked to impulsivity , 2022, Nature Communications.

[14]  E. Viding,et al.  Social thinning and stress generation after childhood maltreatment: a neurocognitive social transactional model of psychiatric vulnerability. , 2022, The lancet. Psychiatry.

[15]  T. Hauser,et al.  Exploration heuristics decrease during youth , 2022, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[16]  S. Constantino,et al.  Suboptimal foraging decisions and involvement of the ventral tegmental area in human opioid addiction , 2022, bioRxiv.

[17]  Z. Kilpatrick,et al.  Stochastic dynamics of social patch foraging decisions , 2022, bioRxiv.

[18]  Sheina Lew‐Levy,et al.  Foraging complexity and the evolution of childhood , 2022, Science advances.

[19]  R. McKay,et al.  Individuals with adverse childhood experiences explore less and underweight reward feedback , 2022, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[20]  Supplemental Material for Examining the Relationship Between Substance Use and Exploration–Exploitation Behavior in Young Adults , 2022, Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology.

[21]  P. Dayan,et al.  Neural encoding of perceived patch value during competitive and hazardous virtual foraging , 2021, Nature Communications.

[22]  W. Bos,et al.  Adolescent risk-taking in the context of exploration and social influence , 2021 .

[23]  Lisa M. Giocomo,et al.  Navigating for reward , 2021, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[24]  T. Hauser,et al.  Children are full of optimism, but those rose-tinted glasses are fading – reduced learning from negative outcomes drives hyperoptimism in children , 2021, bioRxiv.

[25]  Gary A. Kane,et al.  Rat Anterior Cingulate Cortex Continuously Signals Decision Variables in a Patch Foraging Task , 2021, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[26]  Peter B. Jones,et al.  Age at onset of mental disorders worldwide: large-scale meta-analysis of 192 epidemiological studies , 2021, Molecular Psychiatry.

[27]  K. Domschke,et al.  Exploratory drive, fear, and anxiety are dissociable and independent components in foraging mice , 2021, Translational Psychiatry.

[28]  Z. Kilpatrick,et al.  Uncertainty drives deviations in normative foraging decision strategies , 2021, bioRxiv.

[29]  K. Kramer Childhood Teaching and Learning among Savanna Pumé Hunter-Gatherers , 2021, Human Nature.

[30]  A. S. Griffin,et al.  Prey preferences of modern human hunter-gatherers , 2021, Food Webs.

[31]  Ian T. Ruginski,et al.  Harm Avoidance and Mobility During Middle Childhood and Adolescence among Hadza Foragers , 2021, Human Nature.

[32]  A. Migliano,et al.  The origins of human cumulative culture: from the foraging niche to collective intelligence , 2021, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[33]  Charley M. Wu,et al.  Development of directed and random exploration in children. , 2021, Developmental science.

[34]  A. V. van Duijvenvoorde,et al.  Increased Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Activity in Adolescence Benefits Prosocial Reinforcement Learning , 2021, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[35]  Peter Dayan,et al.  Control over patch encounters changes foraging behavior , 2021, iScience.

[36]  Nicolas W. Schuck,et al.  Control over patch encounters changes foraging behaviour , 2021, bioRxiv.

[37]  H. Ferguson,et al.  The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age , 2021, Scientific reports.

[38]  C. Sebastian,et al.  Are Adolescents More Optimal Decision-Makers in Novel Environments? Examining the Benefits of Heightened Exploration in a Patch Foraging Paradigm. , 2020, Developmental science.

[39]  Maria K. Eckstein,et al.  Modeling changes in probabilistic reinforcement learning during adolescence , 2020, bioRxiv.

[40]  Emily G. Liquin,et al.  Children are more exploratory and learn more than adults in an approach-avoid task , 2020, Cognition.

[41]  M. Kavaliers,et al.  Sex and age differences in locomotor and anxiety-like behaviors in rats: From adolescence to adulthood. , 2020, Developmental psychobiology.

[42]  Anne G E Collins,et al.  Activation, but not inhibition, of the indirect pathway disrupts choice rejection in a freely moving, multiple-choice foraging task. , 2020, Cell reports.

[43]  B. Vincent,et al.  Delay discounting and under-valuing of recent information predict poorer adherence to social distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, Scientific Reports.

[44]  P. Hyland,et al.  Pandemic buying: Testing a psychological model of over-purchasing and panic buying using data from the United Kingdom and the Republic of Ireland during the early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic , 2020, PloS one.

[45]  Bolton K. H. Chau,et al.  Global reward state affects learning and activity in raphe nucleus and anterior insula in monkeys , 2020, Nature Communications.

[46]  Ronald E. Dahl,et al.  Reinforcement Learning and Bayesian Inference Provide Complementary Models for the Unique Advantage of Adolescents in Stochastic Reversal , 2020, bioRxiv.

[47]  Vincent D Costa,et al.  Balancing exploration and exploitation with information and randomization , 2020, Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences.

[48]  A. Gopnik Childhood as a solution to explore–exploit tensions , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[49]  S. Perry Behavioural variation and learning across the lifespan in wild white-faced capuchin monkeys , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[50]  Celeste Kidd,et al.  The elaboration of exploratory play , 2020, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B.

[51]  Laura E. Engelhardt,et al.  Genetic Associations Between Executive Functions and a General Factor of Psychopathology. , 2020, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry.

[52]  N. Kolling,et al.  Dopamine Modulates Dynamic Decision-Making during Foraging , 2020, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[53]  M. Moutoussis,et al.  Predictors of risky foraging behaviour in healthy young people , 2020, Nature Human Behaviour.

[54]  Pieter Simoens,et al.  Hybrid foraging in patchy environments using spatial memory , 2020, Journal of the Royal Society Interface.

[55]  R. Dolan,et al.  The role of dopamine in dynamic effort-reward integration , 2020, Neuropsychopharmacology.

[56]  R. Hester,et al.  Transition to substance use disorders: impulsivity for reward and learning from reward , 2019, Social cognitive and affective neuroscience.

[57]  E. Sitnikova,et al.  Developmental milestones and behavior of infant rats: The role of sensory input from whiskers , 2019, Behavioural Brain Research.

[58]  Benjamin R. Eisenreich,et al.  Macaques are risk-averse in a freely moving foraging task , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[59]  Yana Fandakova,et al.  The relationship between pubertal hormones and brain plasticity: Implications for cognitive training in adolescence , 2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[60]  Finnegan J. Calabro,et al.  Development of Hippocampal-Prefrontal Cortex Interactions through Adolescence. , 2019, Cerebral cortex.

[61]  L. Somerville,et al.  Neurodevelopmental shifts in learned value transfer on cognitive control during adolescence , 2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[62]  Ingmar Visser,et al.  Uncertainty-driven regulation of learning and exploration in adolescents: A computational account , 2020, bioRxiv.

[63]  J. Wolfe,et al.  Hybrid foraging search in younger and older age. , 2019, Psychology and aging.

[64]  J. Salamone,et al.  Pharmacological studies of effort-related decision making using mouse touchscreen procedures: effects of dopamine antagonism do not resemble reinforcer devaluation by removal of food restriction , 2019, Psychopharmacology.

[65]  Nathaniel J. Blanco,et al.  Systematic Exploration and Uncertainty Dominate Young Children's Choices. , 2019, Developmental science.

[66]  Andrew R. Mitz,et al.  Subcortical Substrates of Explore-Exploit Decisions in Primates , 2019, Neuron.

[67]  Neil Garrett,et al.  Biased belief updating and suboptimal choice in foraging decisions , 2019, bioRxiv.

[68]  Jiaxin Cindy Tu,et al.  Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex Tracks Multiple Environmental Variables during Search , 2019, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[69]  Samuel J Gershman,et al.  Uncertainty and Exploration , 2018, bioRxiv.

[70]  Anna K. Döring,et al.  When do peers influence adolescent males' risk taking? Examining decision making under conditions of risk and ambiguity , 2019, Journal of Behavioral Decision Making.

[71]  A. Redish,et al.  Learning From Loss After Risk: Dissociating Reward Pursuit and Reward Valuation in a Naturalistic Foraging Task , 2019, Front. Psychiatry.

[72]  Lara M. Wierenga,et al.  A three‐wave longitudinal study of subcortical–cortical resting‐state connectivity in adolescence: Testing age‐ and puberty‐related changes , 2019, Human brain mapping.

[73]  Luis Pacheco-Cobos,et al.  Nahua mushroom gatherers use area-restricted search strategies that conform to marginal value theorem predictions , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[74]  Mehdi Khamassi,et al.  Dopamine blockade impairs the exploration-exploitation trade-off in rats , 2019, Scientific Reports.

[75]  Maria K. Eckstein,et al.  Distentangling the systems contributing to changes in learning during adolescence , 2019, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience.

[76]  Juliano Morimoto,et al.  Foraging decisions as multi-armed bandit problems: Applying reinforcement learning algorithms to foraging data. , 2019, Journal of theoretical biology.

[77]  R. Dahl,et al.  Neural systems underlying reward cue processing in early adolescence: The role of puberty and pubertal hormones , 2019, Psychoneuroendocrinology.

[78]  Hauke R Heekeren,et al.  The computational basis of following advice in adolescents. , 2018, Journal of experimental child psychology.

[79]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  The algorithmic architecture of exploration in the human brain , 2019, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[80]  S. Lissek,et al.  Anxiety Sensitivity and Intolerance of Uncertainty Facilitate Associations Between Generalized Pavlovian Fear and Maladaptive Avoidance Decisions , 2019, Journal of abnormal psychology.

[81]  Abigail A. Scholer,et al.  Self-regulation and the foraging gene (PRKG1) in humans , 2019, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[82]  Benjamin Y. Hayden,et al.  Local field potentials in dorsal anterior cingulate sulcus reflect rewards but not travel time costs during foraging , 2019, Brain and neuroscience advances.

[83]  Samuel J. Gershman,et al.  Dissociable neural correlates of uncertainty underlie different exploration strategies , 2018, bioRxiv.

[84]  B. Luna,et al.  Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition , 2018, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[85]  Alaa A. Ahmed,et al.  Control of movement vigor and decision making during foraging , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[86]  C. Büchel,et al.  Mapping adolescent reward anticipation, receipt, and prediction error during the monetary incentive delay task , 2018, Human brain mapping.

[87]  Ahmed El Hady,et al.  Foraging as an evidence accumulation process , 2018, bioRxiv.

[88]  Jonathan D. Nelson,et al.  Generalization guides human exploration in vast decision spaces , 2017, Nature Human Behaviour.

[89]  M. Hendricks,et al.  Environmental Programming of Adult Foraging Behavior in C. elegans , 2018, Current Biology.

[90]  Peter M. Todd,et al.  Relationship Foraging: Does Time Spent Searching Predict Relationship Length? , 2018, Evolutionary behavioral sciences.

[91]  C. Myers,et al.  Intolerance of uncertainty and conditioned place preference in opioid addiction , 2018, PeerJ.

[92]  Charley M. Wu,et al.  Searching for Rewards Like a Child Means Less Generalization and More Directed Exploration , 2018, bioRxiv.

[93]  Christophe D. Proulx,et al.  A neural pathway controlling motivation to exert effort , 2018, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[94]  K. Dodge,et al.  Age Patterns in Risk Taking Across the World , 2018, Journal of youth and adolescence.

[95]  Elisabeth Schreuders,et al.  Contributions of Reward Sensitivity to Ventral Striatum Activity Across Adolescence and Early Adulthood , 2018, Child development.

[96]  K. Dodge,et al.  Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. , 2018, Developmental science.

[97]  Susan M Sawyer,et al.  The age of adolescence. , 2018, The Lancet. Child & adolescent health.

[98]  K. Ellis-Davies,et al.  How Do Hunter-Gatherer Children Learn Subsistence Skills? , 2017, Human nature.

[99]  N. Kolling,et al.  (Reinforcement?) Learning to forage optimally , 2017, Current Opinion in Neurobiology.

[100]  S. Blakemore,et al.  Age-related differences in social influence on risk perception depend on the direction of influence , 2017, Journal of adolescence.

[101]  Nathaniel D. Daw,et al.  Increased locus coeruleus tonic activity causes disengagement from a patch-foraging task , 2017, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[102]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Increased locus coeruleus tonic activity causes disengagement from a patch-foraging task , 2017, Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience.

[103]  Christopher G. Lucas,et al.  Changes in cognitive flexibility and hypothesis search across human life history from childhood to adolescence to adulthood , 2017, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[104]  Robert C. Wilson,et al.  Charting the Expansion of Strategic Exploratory Behavior During Adolescence , 2017, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[105]  Hongyu Zhao,et al.  A genome-wide gene-by-trauma interaction study of alcohol misuse in two independent cohorts identifies PRKG1 as a risk locus , 2017, Molecular Psychiatry.

[106]  Juliet Y. Davidow,et al.  An Upside to Reward Sensitivity: The Hippocampus Supports Enhanced Reinforcement Learning in Adolescence , 2016, Neuron.

[107]  Aldo A. Faisal,et al.  Internal states drive nutrient homeostasis by modulating exploration-exploitation trade-off , 2016, bioRxiv.

[108]  N. Daw,et al.  Learning the opportunity cost of time in a patch-foraging task , 2015, Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience.

[109]  Pete C. Trimmer,et al.  Adaptive learning can result in a failure to profit from good conditions: implications for understanding depression , 2015, Evolution, medicine, and public health.

[110]  B. Casey Beyond simple models of self-control to circuit-based accounts of adolescent behavior. , 2015, Annual review of psychology.

[111]  P. Hof,et al.  Metabolic costs and evolutionary implications of human brain development , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[112]  Mark A. Straccia,et al.  Anterior Cingulate Engagement in a Foraging Context Reflects Choice Difficulty, Not Foraging Value , 2014, Nature Neuroscience.

[113]  S. Blakemore,et al.  The Developmental Mismatch in Structural Brain Maturation during Adolescence , 2014, Developmental Neuroscience.

[114]  A. Galván,et al.  Neural representation of expected value in the adolescent brain , 2014, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[115]  Alyssa N. Crittenden,et al.  Juvenile foraging among the Hadza: Implications for human life history , 2013 .

[116]  Rui Mata,et al.  Foraging across the life span: is there a reduction in exploration with aging? , 2013, Front. Neurosci..

[117]  J. G. Burns,et al.  Gene–environment interplay in Drosophila melanogaster: Chronic food deprivation in early life affects adult exploratory and fitness traits , 2012, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

[118]  M. Del Giudice,et al.  The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: implications for science, policy, and practice. , 2012, Developmental psychology.

[119]  Timothy E. J. Behrens,et al.  Neural Mechanisms of Foraging , 2012, Science.

[120]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  The central executive as a search process: priming exploration and exploitation across domains. , 2010, Journal of experimental psychology. General.

[121]  Q. D. Walker,et al.  The emergence of gonadal hormone influences on dopaminergic function during puberty , 2010, Hormones and Behavior.

[122]  Russell A. Poldrack,et al.  A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors , 2010, Nature Neuroscience.

[123]  M. Gluck,et al.  Dopaminergic Drugs Modulate Learning Rates and Perseveration in Parkinson's Patients in a Dynamic Foraging Task , 2009, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[124]  V. Manera,et al.  The juvenile transition: A developmental switch point in human life history , 2009 .

[125]  Thomas T. Hills,et al.  Search in External and Internal Spaces , 2008, Psychological science.

[126]  C. D. Gelatt,et al.  Optimization by Simulated Annealing , 1983, Science.

[127]  Charley M. Wu,et al.  Developmental changes in learning resemble stochastic optimization , 2022 .

[128]  Andriy A. Struka,et al.  Self-regulation and the foraging gene ( PRKG 1 ) in humans , 2019 .

[129]  Richard S. Sutton,et al.  Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction , 1998, IEEE Trans. Neural Networks.