The proactive brain and the fate of dead hypotheses

A substantial portion of information flow in the brain is directed top-down, from high processing areas downwards. Signals of this sort are regarded as conveying prior expectations, biasing the processing and eventual perception of incoming stimuli. In this perspective we describe a framework of top-down processing in the visual system in which predictions on the identity of objects in sight aid in their recognition. Focus is placed, in particular, on a relatively uncharted ramification of this framework, that of the fate of initial predictions that are eventually rejected during the process of selection. We propose that such predictions are rapidly inhibited in the brain after a competing option has been selected. Empirical support, along with behavioral, neuronal and computational aspects of this proposal are discussed, and future directions for related research are offered.

[1]  E. Switkes,et al.  Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. V. Spatial frequency , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[2]  A. Buchner,et al.  Negative Priming as a Memory Phenomenon , 2007 .

[3]  J. Changeux,et al.  Experimental and Theoretical Approaches to Conscious Processing , 2011, Neuron.

[4]  N. Logothetis,et al.  Visual competition , 2002, Nature Reviews Neuroscience.

[5]  M Eimer,et al.  A central-peripheral asymmetry in masked priming , 2000, Perception & psychophysics.

[6]  E. Halgren,et al.  Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. , 2006, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.

[7]  Seeing and liking: Biased perception of ambiguous figures consistent with the “inward bias” in aesthetic preferences , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[8]  W. N. Dember,et al.  Mood and global-local visual processing , 1996, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society.

[9]  Marie Bienkowski,et al.  Automatic access of the meanings of ambiguous words in context: Some limitations of knowledge-based processing , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[10]  M. Gernsbacher,et al.  The mechanism of suppression: a component of general comprehension skill. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[11]  S. Tipper Does Negative Priming Reflect Inhibitory Mechanisms? A Review and Integration of Conflicting Views , 2001, The Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. A, Human experimental psychology.

[12]  W. Trammell Neill,et al.  Selective attention and the suppression of cognitive noise. , 1987 .

[13]  Cynthia P. May,et al.  Determinants of negative priming. , 1995, Psychological bulletin.

[14]  John Lisman,et al.  Recognition by top-down and bottom-up processing in cortex: the control of selective attention. , 2003, Journal of neurophysiology.

[15]  M. Bar,et al.  Magnocellular Projections as the Trigger of Top-Down Facilitation in Recognition , 2007, The Journal of Neuroscience.

[16]  T. Robbins,et al.  Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex , 2004, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.

[17]  I. Biederman,et al.  Scene perception: Detecting and judging objects undergoing relational violations , 1982, Cognitive Psychology.

[18]  Yuka Sasaki,et al.  Greater Disruption Due to Failure of Inhibitory Control on an Ambiguous Distractor , 2022 .

[19]  S. Marinakis,et al.  Experimental and theoretical approaches , 2015 .

[20]  H. Barbas,et al.  Anatomic basis of cognitive-emotional interactions in the primate prefrontal cortex , 1995, Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews.

[21]  J. Fuster Prefrontal Cortex , 2018 .

[22]  W. Neill,et al.  Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. , 1992, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.

[23]  E. Switkes,et al.  Functional anatomy of macaque striate cortex. III. Color , 1988, The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience.

[24]  John H. R. Maunsell,et al.  How parallel are the primate visual pathways? , 1993, Annual review of neuroscience.

[25]  D. Hubel,et al.  Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat's visual cortex , 1962, The Journal of physiology.

[26]  M. Eimer,et al.  Response facilitation and inhibition in subliminal priming , 2003, Biological Psychology.

[27]  V. S. Ramachandran,et al.  Perception of shape from shading , 1988, Nature.

[28]  Moshe Bar,et al.  The resilience of object predictions: Early recognition across viewpoints and exemplars , 2014, Psychonomic bulletin & review.

[29]  M. Eimer,et al.  Effects of masked stimuli on motor activation: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence. , 1998, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[30]  N Kanwisher,et al.  Negative priming for spatial locations: identity mismatching, not distractor inhibition. , 1994, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.

[31]  Stanislas Dehaene,et al.  Distinct Brain Mechanisms for Conscious versus Subliminal Error Detection , 2022 .

[32]  M. Bar A Cortical Mechanism for Triggering Top-Down Facilitation in Visual Object Recognition , 2003, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.

[33]  Morton Ann Gernsbacher,et al.  The role of suppression in sentence comprehension , 1991 .

[34]  Francisco Nievas,et al.  Negative priming from the non-selected meaning of the homograph. , 2002, British journal of psychology.

[35]  M. J. Emerson,et al.  The Unity and Diversity of Executive Functions and Their Contributions to Complex “Frontal Lobe” Tasks: A Latent Variable Analysis , 2000, Cognitive Psychology.

[36]  J. Changeux,et al.  A neuronal network model linking subjective reports and objective physiological data during conscious perception , 2003, Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America.