Delayed shape matching benefits from simplicity and symmetry
暂无分享,去创建一个
[1] J. Cutting,et al. Fractal curves and complexity , 1987, Perception & psychophysics.
[2] J. Wagemans. Toward a Better Approach to Goodness: Comments on Van der Helm and Leeuwenberg (1996) , 1999 .
[3] W. Epstein,et al. The status of the minimum principle in the theoretical analysis of visual perception. , 1985, Psychological bulletin.
[4] Donald D. Hoffman,et al. Codon constraints on closed 2D shapes , 1985, Computer Vision Graphics and Image Processing.
[5] S. Edelman,et al. Representation of object similarity in human vision: psychophysics and a computational model , 1998, Vision Research.
[6] R. Luce,et al. Effects of stimulus complexity on mental rotation rate of polygons. , 1987, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[7] J. Hochberg. Effects of the Gestalt revolution: the Cornell symposium on perception. , 1957, Psychology Review.
[8] A. Friedman,et al. Shape discriminations of three-dimensional objects depend on the number and location of bends , 1994, Perception & psychophysics.
[9] M. Tarr,et al. To What Extent Do Unique Parts Influence Recognition Across Changes in Viewpoint? , 1995 .
[10] Johan Wagemans,et al. Characteristics and models of human symmetry detection , 1997, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[11] J Wagemans,et al. Perceptual use of nonaccidental properties. , 1992, Canadian journal of psychology.
[12] Vision Research , 1961, Nature.
[13] L. Cooper,et al. Mental transformations and visual comparison processes: effects of complexity and similarity. , 1976, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[14] S. Edelman,et al. Differential Processing of Objects under Various Viewing Conditions in the Human Lateral Occipital Complex , 1999, Neuron.
[15] F. Attneave,et al. The determination of perceived tridimensional orientation by minimum criteria , 1969 .
[16] J. Wagemans,et al. Segmentation of object outlines into parts: A large-scale integrative study , 2006, Cognition.
[17] Shimon Edelman,et al. Representation and recognition in vision , 1999 .
[18] E. Leeuwenberg. Quantitative specification of information in sequential patterns. , 1969, Psychological review.
[19] Claus Bundesen,et al. Visual pattern matching: Effects of size ratio, complexity, and similarity in simultaneous and successive matching , 1999 .
[20] Johan Wagemans,et al. The concavity effect is a compound of local and global effects , 2007, Perception & psychophysics.
[21] R van Lier,et al. Integrating Global and Local Aspects of Visual Occlusion , 1994, Perception.
[22] I. Biederman,et al. Representation of regular and irregular shapes in macaque inferotemporal cortex. , 2005, Cerebral cortex.
[23] Ravi S. Menon,et al. Differential Effects of Viewpoint on Object-Driven Activation in Dorsal and Ventral Streams , 2002, Neuron.
[24] C. E. Bethell-Fox,et al. Mental rotation: effects of stimulus complexity and familiarity , 1988 .
[25] I. Biederman,et al. Inferior Temporal Neurons Show Greater Sensitivity to Nonaccidental than to Metric Shape Differences , 2001, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience.
[26] R. Henson,et al. Multiple levels of visual object constancy revealed by event-related fMRI of repetition priming , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[27] S F Chipman,et al. Complexity and structure in visual patterns. , 1977, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[28] E. Leeuwenberg,et al. Goodness of visual regularities: a nontransformational approach. , 1996, Psychological review.
[29] H. Barlow. Vision Science: Photons to Phenomenology by Stephen E. Palmer , 2000, Trends in Cognitive Sciences.
[30] B. Gibson,et al. Must Figure-Ground Organization Precede Object Recognition? An Assumption in Peril , 1994 .
[31] R. Vogels,et al. Inferotemporal neurons represent low-dimensional configurations of parameterized shapes , 2001, Nature Neuroscience.
[32] O. Reiser,et al. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[33] Irving Biederman,et al. Pattern goodness and pattern recognition. , 1991 .
[34] J. Hochberg,et al. The effect of landmark features on mental rotation times , 1977, Memory & cognition.
[35] I. Biederman,et al. Tuning for shape dimensions in macaque inferior temporal cortex , 2005, The European journal of neuroscience.
[36] Robert L. Goldstone. Unitization during category learning. , 2000, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[37] D. Perkins,et al. How Good a Bet is Good Form? , 1976, Perception.
[38] K. Koffka. Principles Of Gestalt Psychology , 1936 .
[39] F. Attneave. Some informational aspects of visual perception. , 1954, Psychological review.
[40] I. Biederman,et al. Recognizing depth-rotated objects: Evidence and conditions for three-dimensional viewpoint invariance. , 1993 .
[41] L. Zusne. Visual perception of form , 1970 .
[42] E Leeuwenberg,et al. The perception of foreground and background as derived from structural information theory. , 1984, Acta psychologica.
[43] Helmut Leder,et al. Just how stable are stable aesthetic features? Symmetry, complexity, and the jaws of massive familiarization. , 2009, Acta psychologica.
[44] D. R. Brown,et al. Discrimination and reproduction of patterns: Feature measures and constraint redundancy as predictors , 1969 .
[45] J. Wagemans,et al. Perceptual saliency of points along the contour of everyday objects: A large-scale study , 2008 .
[46] Johan Wagemans,et al. The effect of category learning on the representation of shape: dimensions can be biased but not differentiated. , 2003, Journal of experimental psychology. General.
[47] J. Hochberg,et al. A quantitative approach to figural "goodness". , 1953, Journal of experimental psychology.
[48] J. M. Vanderplas,et al. Complexity, association value, and practice as factors in shape recognition following paired associates training. , 1959, Journal of experimental psychology.
[49] S. Marković,et al. Symmetry, complexity and perceptual economy: Effects of minimum and maximum simplicity conditions , 2001 .
[50] K. Grammer,et al. Facial symmetry and judgements of attractiveness, health and personality , 2006 .
[51] R. Cowie,et al. The perception of structure , 1997 .
[52] I. Biederman. Recognition-by-components: a theory of human image understanding. , 1987, Psychological review.
[53] J. Wagemans,et al. Detection of visual symmetries. , 1995, Spatial vision.
[54] H. J. Clark. Random shape recognition at brief exposure durations , 1968 .
[55] Harrie F J M van Tuijl. Perceptual interpretation of complex line patterns. , 1980 .
[56] E. Leeuwenberg,et al. Coding theory of visual pattern completion. , 1981, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[57] D. Donderi. Visual complexity: a review. , 2006, Psychological bulletin.
[58] T. Poggio,et al. Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex , 1999, Nature Neuroscience.
[59] H. Intraub. Conceptual masking: the effects of subsequent visual events on memory for pictures. , 1984, Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition.
[60] M. Behrmann,et al. Impact of learning on representation of parts and wholes in monkey inferotemporal cortex , 2002, Nature Neuroscience.
[61] J. Pellegrino,et al. Stimulus complexity effects in visual comparisons: the effects of practice and learning context. , 1991, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[62] J. Wagemans. Skewed symmetry: a nonaccidental property used to perceive visual forms. , 1993, Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance.
[63] J. Wagemans,et al. From images to objects: Global and local completions of self-occluded parts , 1999 .
[64] I. Biederman,et al. Shape Tuning in Macaque Inferior Temporal Cortex , 2003, The Journal of Neuroscience.